Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Cognitive neuroscience 2.0: building a cumulative science of human brain function.Tal Yarkoni, Russell A. Poldrack, David C. Van Essen & Tor D. Wager - 2010 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14 (11):489-496.
  • Radical embodiment: Neural dynamics and consciousness.Evan Thompson & Francisco J. Varela - 2001 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5 (10):418-425.
  • Cognitive neuroscience 2.0: building a cumulative science of human brain function.Tor D. Wager Tal Yarkoni, Russell A. Poldrack, David C. Van Essen - 2010 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14 (11):489.
  • Controlled and automatic human information processing: Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory.Richard M. Shiffrin & Walter Schneider - 1977 - Psychological Review 84 (2):128-90.
    Tested the 2-process theory of detection, search, and attention presented by the current authors in a series of experiments. The studies demonstrate the qualitative difference between 2 modes of information processing: automatic detection and controlled search; trace the course of the learning of automatic detection, of categories, and of automatic-attention responses; and show the dependence of automatic detection on attending responses and demonstrate how such responses interrupt controlled processing and interfere with the focusing of attention. The learning of categories is (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   799 citations  
  • Selective visual attention and perceptual coherence.John T. Serences & Steven Yantis - 2006 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10 (1):38-45.
  • The influence of reward associations on conflict processing in the Stroop task.Marty G. Woldorff Ruth M. Krebs, Carsten N. Boehler - 2010 - Cognition 117 (3):341.
  • Arousal, activation, and effort in the control of attention.Karl H. Pribram & Diane McGuinness - 1975 - Psychological Review 82 (2):116-149.
  • Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data?Russell A. Poldrack - 2006 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10 (2):59-63.
  • How do emotion and motivation direct executive control?Luiz Pessoa - 2009 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13 (4):160-166.
  • Resources—a theoretical soup stone?David Navon - 1984 - Psychological Review 91 (2):216-234.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   66 citations  
  • Neuronal representations of cognitive state: reward or attention?John H. R. Maunsell - 2004 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8 (6):261-265.
  • A functional architecture of the human brain: emerging insights from the science of emotion.Kristen A. Lindquist & Lisa Feldman Barrett - 2012 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16 (11):533-540.
  • The influence of reward associations on conflict processing in the Stroop task.Ruth M. Krebs, Carsten N. Boehler & Marty G. Woldorff - 2010 - Cognition 117 (3):341-347.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • How does a brain build a cognitive code?Stephen Grossberg - 1980 - Psychological Review 87 (1):1-51.
  • Salience, relevance, and firing: a priority map for target selection.Jillian H. Fecteau & Douglas P. Munoz - 2006 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10 (8):382-390.
  • Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex.Amit Etkin, Tobias Egner & Raffael Kalisch - 2011 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15 (2):85-93.
  • Suppression of Regional Cerebral Blood during Emotional versus Higher Cognitive Implications for Interactions between Emotion and Cognition.Wayne C. Drevets & Marcus E. Raichle - 1998 - Cognition and Emotion 12 (3):353-385.
    Brain mapping studies using dynamic imaging methods demonstrate areas regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) decreases, as well as areas where increases, during performance of various experimental tasks. Task holds for both sets of cerebral blood flow changes (CBF), providing the opportunity to investigate areas that become and “activated” in the experimental condition relative to control state. Such data yield the intriguing observation that in areas in emotional processing, such as the amygdala, the posteromedial cortex, and the ventral anterior cingulate cortex, (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • A dual-networks architecture of top-down control.Nico U. F. Dosenbach, Damien A. Fair, Alexander L. Cohen, Bradley L. Schlaggar & Steven E. Petersen - 2008 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12 (3):99-105.
  • Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex.George Bush, Phan Luu & Michael I. Posner - 2000 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4 (6):215-222.
    Anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is a part of the brain's limbic system. Classically, this region has been related to affect, on the basis of lesion studies in humans and in animals. In the late 1980s, neuroimaging research indicated that ACC was active in many studies of cognition. The findings from EEG studies of a focal area of negativity in scalp electrodes following an error response led to the idea that ACC might be the brain's error detection and correction device. In (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   138 citations  
  • The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework.Todd S. Braver - 2012 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16 (2):106-113.
  • Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy.Edward Awh, Artem V. Belopolsky & Jan Theeuwes - 2012 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16 (8):437.
    Prominent models of attentional control assert a dichotomy between top-down and bottom-up control, with the former determined by current selection goals and the latter determined by physical salience. This theoretical dichotomy, however, fails to explain a growing number of cases in which neither current goals nor physical salience can account for strong selection biases. For example, equally salient stimuli associated with reward can capture attention, even when this contradicts current selection goals. Thus, although 'top-down' sources of bias are sometimes defined (...)
    Direct download (11 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   93 citations  
  • Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex.Sabine Kastner & Leslie G. Ungerleider - 2000 - Annual Review of Neuroscience 23:315-341.