Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Construal-level theory of psychological distance.Yaacov Trope & Nira Liberman - 2010 - Psychological Review 117 (2):440-463.
    No categories
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   180 citations  
  • “Near” and “far” in language and perception.David Kemmerer - 1999 - Cognition 73 (1):35-63.
  • Demonstratives, joint attention, and the emergence of grammar.Holger Diessel - 2006 - Cognitive Linguistics 17 (4):463-489.
    Drawing on recent work in developmental and comparative psychology, this paper argues that demonstratives function to coordinate the interlocutors' joint focus of attention, which is one of the most basic functions of human communication. The communicative importance of demonstratives is reflected in a number of properties that together characterize them as a particular word class: In contrast to other closed-class expressions, demonstratives are universal, they are generally so old that their roots cannot be traced back to other linguistic items, they (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy.Gergely Csibra - 2010 - Mind and Language 25 (2):141-168.
    I make three related proposals concerning the development of receptive communication in human infants. First, I propose that the presence of communicative intentions can be recognized in others' behaviour before the content of these intentions is accessed or inferred. Second, I claim that such recognition can be achieved by decoding specialized ostensive signals. Third, I argue on empirical bases that, by decoding ostensive signals, human infants are capable of recognizing communicative intentions addressed to them. Thus, learning about actual modes of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   53 citations  
  • Language within your reach: Near–far perceptual space and spatial demonstratives.Kenny R. Coventry, Berenice Valdés, Alejandro Castillo & Pedro Guijarro-Fuentes - 2008 - Cognition 108 (3):889-895.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • Referring as a collaborative process.Herbert H. Clark & Deanna Wilkes-Gibbs - 1986 - Cognition 22 (1):1-39.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   193 citations  
  • Partner‐Specific Adaptation in Dialog.Susan E. Brennan & Joy E. Hanna - 2009 - Topics in Cognitive Science 1 (2):274-291.
    No one denies that people adapt what they say and how they interpret what is said to them, depending on their interactive partners. What is controversial is when and how they do so. Several psycholinguistics research programs have found what appear to be failures to adapt to partners in the early moments of processing and have used this evidence to argue for modularity in the language processing architecture, claiming that the system cannot take into account a partner’s distinct needs or (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  • A kinematic approach to the conceptual representations of this and that.Claudia Bonfiglioli, Chiara Finocchiaro, Benno Gesierich, Francesco Rositani & Massimo Vescovi - 2009 - Cognition 111 (2):270-274.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations