Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Cognition, Construction of Knowledge, and Teaching.Ernst von Glasersfeld - 1989 - Synthese 80 (1):121 - 140.
    The existence of objective knowledge and the possibility of communicating it by means of language have traditionally been taken for granted by educators. Recent developments in the philosophy of science and the historical study of scientific accomplishments have deprived these presuppositions of their former plausibility. Sooner or later, this must have an effect on the teaching of science. In this paper I am presenting a brief outline of an alternative theory of knowing that takes into account the thinking organism's cognitive (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   55 citations  
  • The Impact of Goal Specificity on Strategy Use and the Acquisition of Problem Structure.Regina Vollmeyer, Bruce D. Burns & Keith J. Holyoak - 1996 - Cognitive Science 20 (1):75-100.
    Theories of skill acquisition have made radically different predictions about the role of general problem‐solving methods in acquiring rules that promote effective transfer to new problems. Under one view, methods that focus on reaching specific goals, such as means‐ends analysis, are assumed to provide the basis for efficient knowledge compilation (Anderson, 1987), whereas under an alternative view such methods are believed to disrupt rule induction (Sweller, 1988). We suggest that the role of general methods in learning varies with both the (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Cognitive Load During Problem Solving: Effects on Learning.John Sweller - 1988 - Cognitive Science 12 (2):257-285.
    Considerable evidence indicates that domain specific knowledge in the form of schemas is the primary factor distinguishing experts from novices in problem‐solving skill. Evidence that conventional problem‐solving activity is not effective in schema acquisition is also accumulating. It is suggested that a major reason for the ineffectiveness of problem solving as a learning device, is that the cognitive processes required by the two activities overlap insufficiently, and that conventional problem solving in the form of means‐ends analysis requires a relatively large (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   35 citations  
  • Cognitive Load During Problem Solving: Effects on Learning.John Sweller - 1988 - Cognitive Science 12 (2):257-285.
    Considerable evidence indicates that domain specific knowledge in the form of schemas is the primary factor distinguishing experts from novices in problem‐solving skill. Evidence that conventional problem‐solving activity is not effective in schema acquisition is also accumulating. It is suggested that a major reason for the ineffectiveness of problem solving as a learning device, is that the cognitive processes required by the two activities overlap insufficiently, and that conventional problem solving in the form of means‐ends analysis requires a relatively large (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   78 citations  
  • Expertise and the evolution of consciousness.Matt J. Rossano - 2003 - Cognition 89 (3):207-236.
  • Learning from worked‐out examples: A study on individual differences.Alexander Renkl - 1997 - Cognitive Science 21 (1):1-29.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Learning from Worked-Out Examples: A Study on Individual Differences.Alexander Renkl - 1997 - Cognitive Science 21 (1):1-29.
    The goal of this study was to investigate interindividual differences in learning from worked-out examples with respect to the quality of self-explanations. Restrictions of former studies (e.g., lacking control of time-on-task) were avoided and additional research questions (e.g., reliability and dimensionality of self-explanation characteristics) were addressed. An investigation with 36 university freshmen of education working in individual sessions was conducted. The domain was probability calculus. As predictors of learning, prior knowledge and the quality of self-explanations (thinking aloud protocols) were assessed. (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Goals and Learning in Microworlds.Craig S. Miller, Jill Fain Lehman & Kenneth R. Koedinger - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (3):305-336.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • From implicit skills to explicit knowledge: a bottom‐up model of skill learning.Edward Merrillb & Todd Petersonb - 2001 - Cognitive Science 25 (2):203-244.
    This paper presents a skill learning model CLARION. Different from existing models of mostly high-level skill learning that use a top-down approach (that is, turning declarative knowledge into procedural knowledge through practice), we adopt a bottom-up approach toward low-level skill learning, where procedural knowledge develops first and declarative knowledge develops later. Our model is formed by integrating connectionist, reinforcement, and symbolic learning methods to perform on-line reactive learning. It adopts a two-level dual-representation framework (Sun, 1995), with a combination of localist (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   47 citations  
  • Taking stock of naturalistic decision making.Raanan Lipshitz, Gary Klein, Judith Orasanu & Eduardo Salas - 2001 - Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 14 (5):331-352.
    We review the progress of naturalistic decision making in the decade since the first conference on the subject in 1989. After setting out a brief history of NDM we identify its essential characteristics and consider five of its main contributions: recognition-primed decisions, coping with uncertainty, team decision making, decision errors, and methodology. NDM helped identify important areas of inquiry previously neglected, it introduced new models, conceptualizations, and methods, and recruited applied investigators into the field. Above all, NDM contributed a new (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • Dual Space Search During Scientific Reasoning.David Klahr & Kevin Dunbar - 1988 - Cognitive Science 12 (1):1-48.
    The purpose of the two studies reported here was to develop an integrated model of the scientific reasoning process. Subjects were placed in a simulated scientific discovery context by first teaching them how to use an electronic device and then asking them to discover how a hitherto unencountered function worked. To do this task, subjects had to formulate hypotheses based on their prior knowledge, conduct experiments, and evaluate the results of their experiments. In the first study, using 20 adult subjects, (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   45 citations  
  • Dual Space Search During Scientific Reasoning.David Klahr & Kevin Dunbar - 1988 - Cognitive Science 12 (1):1-48.
    The purpose of the two studies reported here was to develop an integrated model of the scientific reasoning process. Subjects were placed in a simulated scientific discovery context by first teaching them how to use an electronic device and then asking them to discover how a hitherto unencountered function worked. To do this task, subjects had to formulate hypotheses based on their prior knowledge, conduct experiments, and evaluate the results of their experiments. In the first study, using 20 adult subjects, (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   140 citations  
  • Cognition, construction of knowledge, and teaching.Ernst Glasersfeld - 1989 - Synthese 80 (1):121-140.
    The existence of objective knowledge and the possibility of communicating it by means of language have traditionally been taken for granted by educators. Recent developments in the philosophy of science and the historical study of scientific accomplishments have deprived these presuppositions of their former plausibility. Sooner or later, this must have an effect on the teaching of science. In this paper I am presenting a brief outline of an alternative theory of knowing that takes into account the thinking organism''s cognitive (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   51 citations  
  • Dynamic systems as tools for analysing human judgement.Joachim Funke - 2001 - Thinking and Reasoning 7 (1):69 – 89.
    With the advent of computers in the experimental labs, dynamic systems have become a new tool for research on problem solving and decision making. A short review of this research is given and the main features of these systems (connectivity and dynamics) are illustrated. To allow systematic approaches to the influential variables in this area, two formal frameworks (linear structural equations and finite state automata) are presented. Besides the formal background, the article sets out how the task demands of system (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  • The theory of learning by doing.Yuichiro Anzai & Herbert A. Simon - 1979 - Psychological Review 86 (2):124-140.
  • Implicit learning: Below the subjective threshold.Zoltán Dienes & Dianne C. Berry - 1997 - Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 4:3-23.