Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Metamagical Themas: Questing For The Essence Of Mind And Pattern.Douglas Hofstadter - 1996 - Basic Books.
    Hofstadter's collection of quirky essays is unified by its primary concern: to examine the way people perceive and think.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations  
  • The role of theories in conceptual coherence.G. L. Murphy & D. L. Medin - 1999 - In Eric Margolis & Stephen Laurence (eds.), Concepts: Core Readings. MIT Press. pp. 289--316.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   276 citations  
  • Computing machinery and intelligence.Alan M. Turing - 1950 - Mind 59 (October):433-60.
    I propose to consider the question, "Can machines think?" This should begin with definitions of the meaning of the terms "machine" and "think." The definitions might be framed so as to reflect so far as possible the normal use of the words, but this attitude is dangerous, If the meaning of the words "machine" and "think" are to be found by examining how they are commonly used it is difficult to escape the conclusion that the meaning and the answer to (...)
    Direct download (18 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   997 citations  
  • Artificial Intelligence: A Modern Approach.Stuart Jonathan Russell & Peter Norvig (eds.) - 1995 - Prentice-Hall.
    Artificial Intelligence: A Modern Approach, 3e offers the most comprehensive, up-to-date introduction to the theory and practice of artificial intelligence. Number one in its field, this textbook is ideal for one or two-semester, undergraduate or graduate-level courses in Artificial Intelligence. Dr. Peter Norvig, contributing Artificial Intelligence author and Professor Sebastian Thrun, a Pearson author are offering a free online course at Stanford University on artificial intelligence. According to an article in The New York Times, the course on artificial intelligence is (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   269 citations  
  • Word learning as Bayesian inference.Fei Xu & Joshua B. Tenenbaum - 2007 - Psychological Review 114 (2):245-272.
  • One and Done? Optimal Decisions From Very Few Samples.Edward Vul, Noah Goodman, Thomas L. Griffiths & Joshua B. Tenenbaum - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (4):599-637.
    In many learning or inference tasks human behavior approximates that of a Bayesian ideal observer, suggesting that, at some level, cognition can be described as Bayesian inference. However, a number of findings have highlighted an intriguing mismatch between human behavior and standard assumptions about optimality: People often appear to make decisions based on just one or a few samples from the appropriate posterior probability distribution, rather than using the full distribution. Although sampling-based approximations are a common way to implement Bayesian (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   53 citations  
  • Principles of object perception.Elizabeth S. Spelke - 1990 - Cognitive Science 14 (1):29--56.
    Research on human infants has begun to shed light on early-developing processes for segmenting perceptual arrays into objects. Infants appear to perceive objects by analyzing three-dimensional surface arrangements and motions. Their perception does not accord with a general tendency to maximize figural goodness or to attend to nonaccidental geometric relations in visual arrays. Object perception does accord with principles governing the motions of material bodies: Infants divide perceptual arrays into units that move as connected wholes, that move separately from one (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   280 citations  
  • Core knowledge.Elizabeth S. Spelke - 2000 - American Psychologist 55 (11):1233-1243.
    Complex cognitive skills such as reading and calculation and complex cognitive achievements such as formal science and mathematics may depend on a set of building block systems that emerge early in human ontogeny and phylogeny. These core knowledge systems show characteristic limits of domain and task specificity: Each serves to represent a particular class of entities for a particular set of purposes. By combining representations from these systems, however human cognition may achieve extraordinary flexibility. Studies of cognition in human infants (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   208 citations  
  • Vision: Variations on Some Berkeleian Themes.Robert Schwartz & David Marr - 1985 - Philosophical Review 94 (3):411.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   749 citations  
  • The origins of inquiry: inductive inference and exploration in early childhood.Laura Schulz - 2012 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16 (7):382-389.
  • Domain-specific perceptual causality in children depends on the spatio-temporal configuration, not motion onset.Anne Schlottmann, Katy Cole, Rhianna Watts & Marina White - 2013 - Frontiers in Psychology 4.
  • Reconciling intuitive physics and Newtonian mechanics for colliding objects.Adam N. Sanborn, Vikash K. Mansinghka & Thomas L. Griffiths - 2013 - Psychological Review 120 (2):411-437.
  • The perceptron: A probabilistic model for information storage and organization in the brain.F. Rosenblatt - 1958 - Psychological Review 65 (6):386-408.
    If we are eventually to understand the capability of higher organisms for perceptual recognition, generalization, recall, and thinking, we must first have answers to three fundamental questions: 1. How is information about the physical world sensed, or detected, by the biological system? 2. In what form is information stored, or remembered? 3. How does information contained in storage, or in memory, influence recognition and behavior? The first of these questions is in the.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   170 citations  
  • Inductive judgments about natural categories.Lance J. Rips - 1975 - Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 14 (6):665-681.
    The present study examined the effects of semantic structure on simple inductive judgments about category members. For a particular category, subjects were told that one of the species had a given property and were asked to estimate the proportion of instances in the other species that possessed the property. The results indicated that category structure—in particular, the typicality of the species—influenced subjects' judgments. These results were interpreted by models based on the following assumption: When little is known about the underlying (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   78 citations  
  • Causal knowledge and categories: The effects of causal beliefs on categorization, induction, and similarity.Bob Rehder & Reid Hastie - 2001 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 130 (3):323-360.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   44 citations  
  • On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition.Steven Pinker & Alan Prince - 1988 - Cognition 28 (1-2):73-193.
  • On the adequacy of prototype theory as a theory of concepts.Daniel N. Osherson & Edward E. Smith - 1981 - Cognition 9 (1):35-58.
  • The role of theories in conceptual coherence.Gregory L. Murphy & Douglas L. Medin - 1985 - Psychological Review 92 (3):289-316.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   486 citations  
  • Comprehending Complex Concepts.Gregory L. Murphy - 1988 - Cognitive Science 12 (4):529-562.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   44 citations  
  • Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory.James L. McClelland, Bruce L. McNaughton & Randall C. O'Reilly - 1995 - Psychological Review 102 (3):419-457.
  • Letting structure emerge: connectionist and dynamical systems approaches to cognition.James L. McClelland, Matthew M. Botvinick, David C. Noelle, David C. Plaut, Timothy T. Rogers, Mark S. Seidenberg & Linda B. Smith - 2010 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14 (8):348-356.
  • Words and the world: predictive coding and the language-perception-cognition interface.Gary Lupyan & Andy Clark - 2015 - Current Directions in Psychological Science 24 (4):279-284.
    Can what we know change what we see? Does language affect cognition and perception? The last few years have seen increased attention to these seemingly disparate questions, but with little theoretical advance. We argue that substantial clarity can be gained by considering these questions through the lens of predictive processing, a framework in which mental representations—from the perceptual to the cognitive—reflect an interplay between downward-flowing predictions and upward-flowing sensory signals. This framework provides a parsimonious account of how what we know (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   60 citations  
  • How Language Programs the Mind.Gary Lupyan & Benjamin Bergen - 2016 - Topics in Cognitive Science 8 (1):408-424.
    Many animals can be trained to perform novel tasks. People, too, can be trained, but sometime in early childhood people transition from being trainable to something qualitatively more powerful—being programmable. We argue that such programmability constitutes a leap in the way that organisms learn, interact, and transmit knowledge, and that what facilitates or enables this programmability is the learning and use of language. We then examine how language programs the mind and argue that it does so through the manipulation of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • How Language Programs the Mind.Gary Lupyan & Benjamin Bergen - 2016 - Topics in Cognitive Science 8 (2):408-424.
    Many animals can be trained to perform novel tasks. People, too, can be trained, but sometime in early childhood people transition from being trainable to something qualitatively more powerful—being programmable. We argue that such programmability constitutes a leap in the way that organisms learn, interact, and transmit knowledge, and that what facilitates or enables this programmability is the learning and use of language. We then examine how language programs the mind and argue that it does so through the manipulation of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Explanation and categorization: How “why?” informs “what?”.Tania Lombrozo - 2009 - Cognition 110 (2):248-253.
    Recent theoretical and empirical work suggests that explanation and categorization are intimately related. This paper explores the hypothesis that explanations can help structure conceptual representations, and thereby influence the relative importance of features in categorization decisions. In particular, features may be differentially important depending on the role they play in explaining other features or aspects of category membership. Two experiments manipulate whether a feature is explained mechanistically, by appeal to proximate causes, or functionally, by appeal to a function or goal. (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  • Perception of the speech code.A. M. Liberman, F. S. Cooper, D. P. Shankweiler & M. Studdert-Kennedy - 1967 - Psychological Review 74 (6):431-461.
  • Scientific Discovery: Computational Explorations of the Creative Process. Pat Langley, Herbert A. Simon, Gary L. Bradshaw, Jan M. Zytkow.Malcolm R. Forster - 1990 - Philosophy of Science 57 (2):336-338.
  • The Role of Explanation in Discovery and Generalization: Evidence From Category Learning.Joseph J. Williams & Tania Lombrozo - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (5):776-806.
    Research in education and cognitive development suggests that explaining plays a key role in learning and generalization: When learners provide explanations—even to themselves—they learn more effectively and generalize more readily to novel situations. This paper proposes and tests a subsumptive constraints account of this effect. Motivated by philosophical theories of explanation, this account predicts that explaining guides learners to interpret what they are learning in terms of unifying patterns or regularities, which promotes the discovery of broad generalizations. Three experiments provide (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   36 citations  
  • A decision network account of reasoning about other people’s choices.Alan Jern & Charles Kemp - 2015 - Cognition 142 (C):12-38.
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Children’s understanding of the costs and rewards underlying rational action.Julian Jara-Ettinger, Hyowon Gweon, Joshua B. Tenenbaum & Laura E. Schulz - 2015 - Cognition 140 (C):14-23.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Letting Structure Emerge: Connectionist and Dynamical Systems Approaches to Cognition.Linda B. Smith James L. McClelland, Matthew M. Botvinick, David C. Noelle, David C. Plaut, Timothy T. Rogers, Mark S. Seidenberg - 2010 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14 (8):348.
  • Dynamic binding in a neural network for shape recognition.John E. Hummel & Irving Biederman - 1992 - Psychological Review 99 (3):480-517.
  • Parts of recognition.D. D. Hoffman & W. A. Richards - 1984 - Cognition 18 (1-3):65-96.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   144 citations  
  • Young infants’ actions reveal their developing knowledge of support variables: Converging evidence for violation-of-expectation findings.Susan J. Hespos & Renée Baillargeon - 2008 - Cognition 107 (1):304-316.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • A Cognitive Model of Planning.Barbara Hayes-Roth & Frederick Hayes-Roth - 1979 - Cognitive Science 3 (4):275-310.
    This paper presents a cognitive model of the planning process. The model generalizes the theoretical architecture of the Hearsay‐ll system. Thus, it assumes that planning comprises the activities of a variety of cognitive “specialists.” Each specialist can suggest certain kinds of decisions for incorporation into the plan in progress. These include decisions about: (a) how to approach the planning problem; (b) what knowledge bears on the problem; (c) what kinds of actions to try to plan; (d) what specific actions to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   109 citations  
  • The formation of learning sets.Harry F. Harlow - 1949 - Psychological Review 56 (1):51-65.
  • A Theory of Causal Learning in Children: Causal Maps and Bayes Nets.Alison Gopnik, Clark Glymour, Laura Schulz, Tamar Kushnir & David Danks - 2004 - Psychological Review 111 (1):3-32.
    We propose that children employ specialized cognitive systems that allow them to recover an accurate “causal map” of the world: an abstract, coherent, learned representation of the causal relations among events. This kind of knowledge can be perspicuously understood in terms of the formalism of directed graphical causal models, or “Bayes nets”. Children’s causal learning and inference may involve computations similar to those for learning causal Bayes nets and for predicting with them. Experimental results suggest that 2- to 4-year-old children (...)
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   229 citations  
  • Monte-Carlo tree search and rapid action value estimation in computer Go.Sylvain Gelly & David Silver - 2011 - Artificial Intelligence 175 (11):1856-1875.
  • Dynamic mental representations.Jennifer J. Freyd - 1987 - Psychological Review 94 (4):427-438.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   83 citations  
  • The Language of Thought. [REVIEW]Adam Morton - 1978 - Journal of Philosophy 75 (3):161-169.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   104 citations  
  • Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis.Jerry A. Fodor & Zenon W. Pylyshyn - 1988 - Cognition 28 (1-2):3-71.
    This paper explores the difference between Connectionist proposals for cognitive a r c h i t e c t u r e a n d t h e s o r t s o f m o d e l s t hat have traditionally been assum e d i n c o g n i t i v e s c i e n c e . W e c l a i m t h a t t h (...)
    Direct download (13 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1119 citations  
  • Goal attribution to inanimate agents by 6.5-month-old infants.Gergely Csibra - 2008 - Cognition 107 (2):705-717.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   56 citations  
  • One‐year‐old infants use teleological representations of actions productively.Gergely Csibra, Szilvia Bíró, Orsolya Koós & György Gergely - 2003 - Cognitive Science 27 (1):111-133.
    Two experiments investigated whether infants represent goal‐directed actions of others in a way that allows them to draw inferences to unobserved states of affairs (such as unseen goal states or occluded obstacles). We measured looking times to assess violation of infants' expectations upon perceiving either a change in the actions of computer‐animated figures or in the context of such actions. The first experiment tested whether infants would attribute a goal to an action that they had not seen completed. The second (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   62 citations  
  • Where science starts: Spontaneous experiments in preschoolers’ exploratory play.Claire Cook, Noah D. Goodman & Laura E. Schulz - 2011 - Cognition 120 (3):341-349.
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • Cognitive control over learning: Creating, clustering, and generalizing task-set structure.Anne G. E. Collins & Michael J. Frank - 2013 - Psychological Review 120 (1):190-229.
  • Creativity and artificial intelligence.Margaret A. Boden - 1998 - Artificial Intelligence 103 (1-2):347-356.
  • An Overview of KRL, a Knowledge Representation Language.Daniel G. Bobrow & Terry Winograd - 1977 - Cognitive Science 1 (1):3-46.
    This paper describes KRL, a Knowledge Representation Language designed for use in understander systems. It outlines both the general concepts which underlie our research and the details of KRL‐0, an experimental implementation of some of these concepts. KRL is an attempt to integrate procedural knowledge with a broad base of declarative forms. These forms provide a variety of ways to express the logical structure of the knowledge, in order to give flexibility in associating procedures (for memory and reasoning) with specific (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   99 citations  
  • Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding.Irving Biederman - 1987 - Psychological Review 94 (2):115-147.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   534 citations  
  • A constructivist connectionist model of transitions on false-belief tasks.Vincent G. Berthiaume, Thomas R. Shultz & Kristine H. Onishi - 2013 - Cognition 126 (3):441-458.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Action understanding as inverse planning.Chris L. Baker, Rebecca Saxe & Joshua B. Tenenbaum - 2009 - Cognition 113 (3):329-349.
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   84 citations