Brain-behavioral studies: The importance of staying close to the data

Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):497-514 (1981)
  Copy   BIBTEX

Abstract

This article has no associated abstract. (fix it)

Links

PhilArchive



    Upload a copy of this work     Papers currently archived: 91,386

External links

Setup an account with your affiliations in order to access resources via your University's proxy server

Through your library

Similar books and articles

Brain scaling, behavioral ability, and human evolution.P. Thomas Schoenemann - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (2):293-295.
Hemineglect, extinction, and the importance of conscious processing.Eric Siéroff - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (3):354-355.
ERPs and the modularity of cognitive processes.Valerie Gray Hardcastle - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (3):520-521.
Beyond response output: More logical than we think.Wim De Neys - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (1):87-88.
Individual data and clear assumptions about movement.Jan E. Holly - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (2):313-314.
Temporal perception: A key to understanding language.Elzbieta Szelag & Ernst Pöppel - 2000 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (1):52-52.

Analytics

Added to PP
2013-11-21

Downloads
52 (#300,476)

6 months
24 (#113,849)

Historical graph of downloads
How can I increase my downloads?

References found in this work

The Study of Instinct.N. Tinbergen - 1954 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 5 (17):72-76.
Are theories of learning necessary?B. F. Skinner - 1950 - Psychological Review 57 (4):193-216.
Essay on mind.Donald Olding Hebb - 1980 - Hillsdale, N.J.: L. Erlbaum Associates.

View all 16 references / Add more references