Time Course of Creativity in Dance

Frontiers in Psychology 11 (2020)
  Copy   BIBTEX

Abstract

Time-motion studies revolutionized the design and efficiency of repetitive work last century. Would time-idea studies revolutionize the rules of intellectual/creative work this century? Collaborating with seven professional dancers, we set out to discover if there were any significant temporal patterns to be found in a timeline coded to show when dancers come up with ideas and when they modify or reject them. On each of 3 days, the dancers were given a choreographic problem to help them generate a novel, high quality contemporary dance phrase. They were videoed as they worked on this task for sessions of 15, 30, and 45 min. At the end of each 15 min interval during each session, we had them perform the phrase they were creating. They recorded and then coded the video of themselves dancing during these sessions by using a coding language we developed with them to identify when ideas are introduced, modified, and rejected. We found that most ideas are created early and that though these early ideas are aggressively pruned early on, many still make it into the final product. The two competing accounts of creativity in design research make predictions for the temporal structure of creativity. Our results support neither account, rather showing a more blended version of the two. The iterative design view, arguably the dominant view, is that good ideas are the product of generating many ideas, choosing one fairly early, committing to it, and iteratively improving it. The “fail fast fail often” view is that good ideas are the product of rapidly generating and discarding ideas and holding back from early commitment to any one in particular. The result of holding back commitment, typically, is not that an idea is taken up later and then incrementally improved at the last minute, as much as that later designs are not completely novel, instead incorporating the best parts of the entire sequence of ideas. In our study, we found no evidence that one account or the other was more predictive for the domain of contemporary dance. The behavior of the dancers that we studied revealed elements of both, calling into question how predictive these theories are.

Links

PhilArchive



    Upload a copy of this work     Papers currently archived: 91,202

External links

Setup an account with your affiliations in order to access resources via your University's proxy server

Through your library

Similar books and articles

Creative Cognition in Choreography.David Kirsh - 2011 - Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Computational Creativity:1-6.
Dance Improvisational Cognition.Klara Łucznik & Frank Loesche - 2017 - Avant: Trends in Interdisciplinary Studies 8 (T):227-238.
Doesn't a dance require dancers?N. S. Thompson & Jaan Valsiner - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (5):641-642.
„They Never Dance”: The Choreography of Le Sacre du Printemps, 1913.Hanna Järvinen - 2013 - Avant: Trends in Interdisciplinary Studies 4 (3):69–108.
Creative Cognition in Choreography.David Kirsh - 2011 - Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Computational Creatifity.
Précis of The creative mind: Myths and mechanisms.Margaret A. Boden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):519-531.
Foregrounding the Imagination: Re-reflecting on Dancers’ Engagement with Video Self-recordings.Shantel Ehrenberg - 2019 - In Stuart Grant, Jodie McNeilly-Renaudie & Matthew Wagner (eds.), Performance Phenomenology: To the Thing Itself. Springer Verlag. pp. 133-163.

Analytics

Added to PP
2021-01-11

Downloads
14 (#930,021)

6 months
5 (#526,961)

Historical graph of downloads
How can I increase my downloads?

Author's Profile

David Kirsh
University of California, San Diego

Citations of this work

No citations found.

Add more citations