Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Relative cue precision and prior knowledge contribute to the preference of proximal and distal landmarks in human orientation.Yafei Qi & Weimin Mou - 2024 - Cognition 247 (C):105772.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A Cultural Species and its Cognitive Phenotypes: Implications for Philosophy.Joseph Henrich, Damián E. Blasi, Cameron M. Curtin, Helen Elizabeth Davis, Ze Hong, Daniel Kelly & Ivan Kroupin - 2022 - Review of Philosophy and Psychology 14 (2):349-386.
    After introducing the new field of cultural evolution, we review a growing body of empirical evidence suggesting that culture shapes what people attend to, perceive and remember as well as how they think, feel and reason. Focusing on perception, spatial navigation, mentalizing, thinking styles, reasoning (epistemic norms) and language, we discuss not only important variation in these domains, but emphasize that most researchers (including philosophers) and research participants are psychologically peculiar within a global and historical context. This rising tide of (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Spatial updating according to a fixed reference direction of a briefly viewed layout.Hui Zhang, Weimin Mou & Timothy P. McNamara - 2011 - Cognition 119 (3):419-429.
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Non-spatial similarity can bias spatial distances in a cognitive map.Xing Xing & Jeffrey A. Saunders - 2022 - Cognition 229 (C):105251.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Challenges for identifying the neural mechanisms that support spatial navigation: the impact of spatial scale.Thomas Wolbers & Jan M. Wiener - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8.
  • Human navigation in curved spaces.Christopher Widdowson & Ranxiao Frances Wang - 2022 - Cognition 218 (C):104923.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Isolating observer-based reference directions in human spatial memory: Head, body, and the self-to-array axis.David Waller, Yvonne Lippa & Adam Richardson - 2008 - Cognition 106 (1):157-183.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Conceptuality in spatial representations.Gottfried Vosgerau - 2007 - Philosophical Psychology 20 (3):349 – 365.
    The notion of conceptuality is still unclear and vague. I will present a definition of conceptual and nonconceptual representations that is grounded in different aspects of the representations’ structures. This definition is then used to interpret empirical results from human and animal navigation. It will be shown, that the distinction between egocentric and allocentric spatial representations can be matched onto the conceptual vs. nonconceptual distinction. The phenomena discussed in spatial navigation are thereby put into a wider context of cognitive abilities, (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Animals' use of landmarks and metric information to reorient: effects of the size of the experimental space.Valeria Anna Sovrano, Angelo Bisazza & Giorgio Vallortigara - 2005 - Cognition 97 (2):121-133.
  • Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? Insights from a rodent navigation model.Denis Sheynikhovich, Ricardo Chavarriaga, Thomas Strösslin, Angelo Arleo & Wulfram Gerstner - 2009 - Psychological Review 116 (3):540-566.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Where you are affects what you can easily imagine: Environmental geometry elicits sensorimotor interference in remote perspective taking.Bernhard E. Riecke & Timothy P. McNamara - 2017 - Cognition 169 (C):1-14.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Where am I? Who am I? The Relation Between Spatial Cognition, Social Cognition and Individual Differences in the Built Environment.Michael J. Proulx, Orlin S. Todorov, Amanda Taylor Aiken & Alexandra A. de Sousa - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
  • Measuring Spontaneous Focus on Space in Preschool Children.Jasmin Perez & Koleen McCrink - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Ambient visual information confers a context-specific, long-term benefit on memory for haptic scenes.Achille Pasqualotto, Ciara M. Finucane & Fiona N. Newell - 2013 - Cognition 128 (3):363-379.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Burge on perception and sensation.Lauren Olin - 2016 - Synthese 193 (5):1479-1508.
    In Origins of Objectivity Burge advances a theory of perception according to which perceptions are, themselves, objective representations. The possession of veridicality conditions by perceptual states—roughly, non-propositional analogues of truth-conditions—is central to Burge’s account of how perceptual states differ, empirically and metaphysically, from sensory states. Despite an impressive examination of the relevant empirical literatures, I argue here that Burge has not succeeded in securing a distinction between perception and “mere” sensation.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • A viewpoint-independent process for spatial reorientation.Marko Nardini, Rhiannon L. Thomas, Victoria C. P. Knowland, Oliver J. Braddick & Janette Atkinson - 2009 - Cognition 112 (2):241-248.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Reference directions and reference objects in spatial memory of a briefly viewed layout.Weimin Mou, Chengli Xiao & Timothy P. McNamara - 2008 - Cognition 108 (1):136-154.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Object location memory: Integration and competition between multiple context objects but not between observers’ body and context objects.Weimin Mou & Marcia L. Spetch - 2013 - Cognition 126 (2):181-197.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Intrinsic frames of reference and egocentric viewpoints in scene recognition.Weimin Mou, Yanli Fan, Timothy P. McNamara & Charles B. Owen - 2008 - Cognition 106 (2):750-769.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Dissociating position and heading estimations: Rotated visual orientation cues perceived after walking reset headings but not positions.Weimin Mou & Lei Zhang - 2014 - Cognition 133 (3):553-571.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Working Memory in Wayfinding—A Dual Task Experiment in a Virtual City.Tobias Meilinger, Markus Knauff & Heinrich H. Bülthoff - 2008 - Cognitive Science 32 (4):755-770.
  • The First-Person Perspective Is Not a Defining Feature of Consciousness.Dylan Ludwig - 2021 - Dialogue 60 (3):435-446.
    RésuméLes philosophes et les scientifiques présument en général que la conscience est caractérisée par « un point de vue à la première personne ». Selon une interprétation de cette revendication, les expériences sont définies, au moins en partie, par des représentations qui encodent un « point de vue » centré sur le sujet. Par contre, les revendications sur les caractéristiques déterminantes de la conscience doivent être attentives à la possibilité d'une dissociation : si une structure neurobiologique ou une fonction psychologique (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Dual Systems for Spatial Updating in Immediate and Retrieved Environments: Evidence from Bias Analysis.Chuanjun Liu & Chengli Xiao - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Retrieving enduring spatial representations after disorientation.Xiaoou Li, Weimin Mou & Timothy P. McNamara - 2012 - Cognition 124 (2):143-155.
  • Competing perspectives on frames of reference in language and thought.Peggy Li & Linda Abarbanell - 2018 - Cognition 170 (C):9-24.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Navigation as a source of geometric knowledge: Young children’s use of length, angle, distance, and direction in a reorientation task.Sang Ah Lee, Valeria A. Sovrano & Elizabeth S. Spelke - 2012 - Cognition 123 (1):144-161.
  • Spatial memory in foraging games.Bryan E. Kerster, Theo Rhodes & Christopher T. Kello - 2016 - Cognition 148:85-96.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Navigating in a three-dimensional world.Kathryn J. Jeffery, Aleksandar Jovalekic, Madeleine Verriotis & Robin Hayman - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (5):523-543.
    The study of spatial cognition has provided considerable insight into how animals (including humans) navigate on the horizontal plane. However, the real world is three-dimensional, having a complex topography including both horizontal and vertical features, which presents additional challenges for representation and navigation. The present article reviews the emerging behavioral and neurobiological literature on spatial cognition in non-horizontal environments. We suggest that three-dimensional spaces are represented in a quasi-planar fashion, with space in the plane of locomotion being computed separately and (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • How toddlers represent enclosed spaces.Janellen Huttenlocher & Marina Vasilyeva - 2003 - Cognitive Science 27 (5):749-766.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Spatial Updating according to a Fixed Reference Direction of a Briefly Viewed Layout.Timothy P. McNamara Hui Zhang, Weimin Mou - 2011 - Cognition 119 (3):419.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Children and Adults Prefer the Egocentric Representation to the Allocentric Representation.Qingfen Hu, Ying Yang, Zhenzhen Huang & Yi Shao - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cognitive Artifacts for Geometric Reasoning.Mateusz Hohol & Marcin Miłkowski - 2019 - Foundations of Science 24 (4):657-680.
    In this paper, we focus on the development of geometric cognition. We argue that to understand how geometric cognition has been constituted, one must appreciate not only individual cognitive factors, such as phylogenetically ancient and ontogenetically early core cognitive systems, but also the social history of the spread and use of cognitive artifacts. In particular, we show that the development of Greek mathematics, enshrined in Euclid’s Elements, was driven by the use of two tightly intertwined cognitive artifacts: the use of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Geometric determinants of human spatial memory.Tom Hartley, Iris Trinkler & Neil Burgess - 2004 - Cognition 94 (1):39-75.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Counterexample Search in Diagram‐Based Geometric Reasoning.Yacin Hamami, John Mumma & Marie Amalric - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (4):e12959.
    Topological relations such as inside, outside, or intersection are ubiquitous to our spatial thinking. Here, we examined how people reason deductively with topological relations between points, lines, and circles in geometric diagrams. We hypothesized in particular that a counterexample search generally underlies this type of reasoning. We first verified that educated adults without specific math training were able to produce correct diagrammatic representations contained in the premisses of an inference. Our first experiment then revealed that subjects who correctly judged an (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Language and memory for object location.Harmen B. Gudde, Kenny R. Coventry & Paul E. Engelhardt - 2016 - Cognition 153 (C):99-107.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Are All Spatial Reference Frames Egocentric? Reinterpreting Evidence for Allocentric, Object-Centered, or World-Centered Reference Frames.Flavia Filimon - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  • Geometric and featural systems, separable and combined: Evidence from reorientation in people with Williams syndrome.Katrina Ferrara & Barbara Landau - 2015 - Cognition 144 (C):123-133.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Transfer of route learning from virtual to real environments.Martin J. Farrell, Paul Arnold, Steve Pettifer, Jessica Adams, Tom Graham & Michael MacManamon - 2003 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 9 (4):219.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Consciousness, art, and the brain: Lessons from Marcel Proust.Russell Epstein - 2004 - Consciousness and Cognition 13 (2):213-40.
    In his novel Remembrance of Things Past, Marcel Proust argues that conventional descriptions of the phenomenology of consciousness are incomplete because they focus too much on the highly-salient sensory information that dominates each moment of awareness and ignore the network of associations that lies in the background. In this paper, I explicate Proust’s theory of conscious experience and show how it leads him directly to a theory of aesthetic perception. Proust’s division of awareness into two components roughly corresponds to William (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • The representation selection problem: Why we should favor the geometric-module framework of spatial reorientation over the view-matching framework.Alexandre Duval - 2019 - Cognition 192 (C):103985.
    Many species rely on the three-dimensional surface layout of an environment to find a desired goal following disorientation. They generally do so to the exclusion of other important spatial cues. Two influential frameworks for explaining that phenomenon are provided by geometric-module theories and view-matching theories of reorientation respectively. The former posit a module that operates only on representations of the global geo- metry of three-dimensional surfaces to guide behavior. The latter place snapshots, stored representations of the subject’s two-dimensional retinal stimulation (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Isolating observer-based reference directions in human spatial memory: Head, body, and the self-to-array axis.Adam Richardson David Waller, Yvonne Lippa - 2008 - Cognition 106 (1):157.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Doxastic Revision in Non-Human Animals: The First-Order Model.Laura Danón & Daniel E. Kalpokas - forthcoming - Review of Philosophy and Psychology:1-22.
    If we focus on current debates on how creatures revise or correct their beliefs, we can identify two opposing approaches that we propose to call “intellectualism” and “minimalism.” In this paper, we outline a new account of doxastic revision — “the first-order model”— that is neither as cognitively demanding as intellectualism nor as deflationary as minimalism. First-order doxastic revision, we argue, is a personal-level process in which a creature rejects some beliefs and accepts others based on reasons. However, it does (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Episodic memory for human-like agents and human-like agents for episodic memory.Cyril Brom, Jiří Lukavský & Rudolf Kadlec - 2010 - International Journal of Machine Consciousness 2 (2):227-244.
  • Spatial memory: how egocentric and allocentric combine.Neil Burgess - 2006 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10 (12):551-557.
  • Orientational manoeuvres in the dark: dissociating allocentric and egocentric influences on spatial memory.N. Burgess, H. Spiers & E. PalEologou - 2004 - Cognition 94 (2):149-166.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  • The spatial layout of doorways and environmental boundaries shape the content of event memories.Matthew G. Buckley, Liam A. M. Myles, Alexander Easton & Anthony McGregor - 2022 - Cognition 225 (C):105091.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Representational flexibility and specificity following spatial descriptions of real-world environments.Tad T. Brunyé, David N. Rapp & Holly A. Taylor - 2008 - Cognition 108 (2):418-443.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Topographical Disorientation: Clinical and Theoretical Significance of Long-Lasting Improvements Following Imagery-Based Training.Maddalena Boccia, Alessia Bonavita, Sofia Diana, Antonella Di Vita, Maria Paola Ciurli & Cecilia Guariglia - 2019 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13.
  • Marcus Giaquinto. Visual thinking in mathematics: An epistemological study. [REVIEW]Jeremy Avigad - 2009 - Philosophia Mathematica 17 (1):95-108.
    Published in 1891, Edmund Husserl's first book, Philosophie der Arithmetik, aimed to ‘prepare the scientific foundations for a future construction of that discipline’. His goals should seem reasonable to contemporary philosophers of mathematics: "…through patient investigation of details, to seek foundations, and to test noteworthy theories through painstaking criticism, separating the correct from the erroneous, in order, thus informed, to set in their place new ones which are, if possible, more adequately secured. 1"But the ensuing strategy for grounding mathematical knowledge (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • A matter of trust: when landmarks and geometry are used during reorientation.Kristin R. Ratliff & Nora S. Newcombe - 2007 - In McNamara D. S. & Trafton J. G. (eds.), Proceedings of the 29th Annual Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society. pp. 581.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark