Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Adaptation and attention.Steven W. Zucker - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):458-458.
  • Visual Search for Wines with a Triangle on the Label in a Virtual Store.Hui Zhao, Fuxing Huang, Charles Spence & Xiaoang Wan - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A theory of eye movements during target acquisition.Gregory J. Zelinsky - 2008 - Psychological Review 115 (4):787-835.
  • Appraising the role of visual threat in speeded detection and classification tasks.Yue Yue & Philip T. Quinlan - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Attention to detail?Malcolm P. Young, Ian R. Paterson & David I. Perrett - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):417-418.
  • Complexity, guided search, and the data.Jeremy M. Wolfe - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):457-458.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Where's the psychological reality?C. Philip Winder - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):417-417.
  • Preattentive analysis of facial expressions of emotion.Murray White - 1995 - Cognition and Emotion 9 (5):439-460.
  • Emotional triangles: A test of emotion-based attentional capture by simple geometric shapes.Derrick G. Watson, Elisabeth Blagrove & Sally Selwood - 2011 - Cognition and Emotion 25 (7):1149-1164.
  • Is extension to perception of real-world objects and scenes possible?J. Wagemans, K. Verfaillie, P. De Graef & K. Lamberts - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):415-417.
  • Is the exogenous orienting of spatial attention truly automatic? Evidence from unimodal and multisensory studies.Valerio Santangelo & Charles Spence - 2008 - Consciousness and Cognition 17 (3):989-1015.
    The last decade has seen great progress in the study of the nature of crossmodal links in exogenous and endogenous spatial attention . Exogenous spatial cuing studies of human crossmodal attention and multisensory integration. In C. Spence, & J. Driver , Crossmodal space and crossmodal attention . Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.], for a recent review). A growing body of research now highlights the existence of robust crossmodal links between auditory, visual, and tactile spatial attention. However, until recently, studies of (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • On brains and models.William R. Uttal - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):456-457.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Some important constraints on complexity.Leonard Uhr - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):455-456.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cultural differences in visual search for geometric figures.Yoshiyuki Ueda, Lei Chen, Jonathon Kopecky, Emily S. Cramer, Ronald A. Rensink, David E. Meyer, Shinobu Kitayama & Jun Saiki - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (1):286-310.
    While some studies suggest cultural differences in visual processing, others do not, possibly because the complexity of their tasks draws upon high-level factors that could obscure such effects. To control for this, we examined cultural differences in visual search for geometric figures, a relatively simple task for which the underlying mechanisms are reasonably well known. We replicated earlier results showing that North Americans had a reliable search asymmetry for line length: Search for long among short lines was faster than vice (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Analyzing vision at the complexity level.John K. Tsotsos - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):423-445.
    The general problem of visual search can be shown to be computationally intractable in a formal, complexity-theoretic sense, yet visual search is extensively involved in everyday perception, and biological systems manage to perform it remarkably well. Complexity level analysis may resolve this contradiction. Visual search can be reshaped into tractability through approximations and by optimizing the resources devoted to visual processing. Architectural constraints can be derived using the minimum cost principle to rule out a large class of potential solutions. The (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   154 citations  
  • A little complexity analysis goes a long way.John K. Tsotsos - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):458-469.
  • Search and the detection and integration of features.Anne Treisman - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):454-455.
  • Is the tag necessary?Ron Sun & Emmanuel Schalit - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):415-415.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The value of modeling visual attention.Gary W. Strong & Bruce A. Whitehead - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):419-433.
  • A solution to the tag-assignment problem for neural networks.Gary W. Strong & Bruce A. Whitehead - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):381-397.
    Purely parallel neural networks can model object recognition in brief displays – the same conditions under which illusory conjunctions have been demonstrated empirically. Correcting errors of illusory conjunction is the “tag-assignment” problem for a purely parallel processor: the problem of assigning a spatial tag to nonspatial features, feature combinations, and objects. This problem must be solved to model human object recognition over a longer time scale. Our model simulates both the parallel processes that may underlie illusory conjunctions and the serial (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   175 citations  
  • Algorithmic complexity analysis does not apply to behaving organisms.Gary W. Strong - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):453-454.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is it really that complex? After all, there are no green elephants.Ralph M. Siegel - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):453-453.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Controlled & automatic processing: behavior, theory, and biological mechanisms.Walter Schneider & Jason M. Chein - 2003 - Cognitive Science 27 (3):525-559.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   41 citations  
  • An attentional hierarchy.Peter A. Sandon - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):414-415.
  • Preemption effects in visual search: Evidence for low-level grouping.Ronald A. Rensink & James T. Enns - 1995 - Psychological Review 102 (1):101-130.
    Experiments are presented showing that visual search for Mueller-Lyer (ML) stimuli is based on complete configurations, rather than component segments. Segments easily detected in isolation were difficult to detect when embedded in a configuration, indicating preemption by low-level groups. This preemption—which caused stimulus components to become inaccessible to rapid search—was an all-or-nothing effect, and so could serve as a powerful test of grouping. It is shown that these effects are unlikely to be due to blurring by simple spatial filters at (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  • Attending to emotional expressions: no evidence for automatic capture in the dot-probe task.Swantje Puls & Klaus Rothermund - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 32 (3):450-463.
    Research on automatic attention to emotional faces offers mixed results. Therefore we examined validity effects for facial expressions of different emotions with a dot-probe paradigm in seven studies. Systematic variations of type of emotion, CTI, task, cue size, and masking allow for a differentiated assessment of attentional capture by emotions and possible moderating factors. Results indicate a general absence of emotional validity effects as well as a lack of significant interactions with either of the manipulated factors, indicating that facial expressions (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Damn the (behavioral) data, full steam ahead.William Prinzmetal & Richard Ivry - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):413-414.
  • Neural networks and computational theory: Solving the right problem.David C. Plaut - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):411-413.
  • Distractor context manipulation in visual search: How expectations modulate proactive control.Marco A. Petilli, Francesco Marini & Roberta Daini - 2020 - Cognition 196 (C):104129.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Simultaneous processing of features may not be possible.D. M. Parker - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):411-411.
  • Tight and loose are not created equal: An asymmetry underlying the representation of fit in English- and Korean-speakers.Heather M. Norbury, Sandra R. Waxman & Hyun-Joo Song - 2008 - Cognition 109 (3):316-325.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Inhibition drives configural superiority of illusory Gestalt: Combined behavioral and drift–diffusion model evidence.Qi-Yang Nie, Mara Maurer, Hermann J. Müller & Markus Conci - 2016 - Cognition 150 (C):150-162.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Support for an intermediate pictorial representation.Michael Mohnhaupt & Bernd Neumann - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):452-453.
  • Fundamental design limitations in tag assignment.Hermann J. Müller, Glyn W. Humphreys, Philip T. Quinlan & Nick Donnelly - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):410-411.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Finding an emotional face in a crowd: Emotional and perceptual stimulus factors influence visual search efficiency.Daniel Lundqvist, Neil Bruce & Arne Öhman - 2015 - Cognition and Emotion 29 (4):621-633.
  • Probability theory as an alternative to complexity.David G. Lowe - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):451-452.
  • An instance theory of attention and memory.Gordon D. Logan - 2002 - Psychological Review 109 (2):376-400.
  • Searching for emotion or race: Task-irrelevant facial cues have asymmetrical effects.Ottmar V. Lipp, Belinda M. Craig, Mareka J. Frost, Deborah J. Terry & Joanne R. Smith - 2014 - Cognition and Emotion 28 (6):1100-1109.
  • Interface transparency and the psychosemantics of most.Jeffrey Lidz, Paul Pietroski, Tim Hunter & Justin Halberda - 2011 - Natural Language Semantics 19 (3):227-256.
    This paper proposes an Interface Transparency Thesis concerning how linguistic meanings are related to the cognitive systems that are used to evaluate sentences for truth/falsity: a declarative sentence S is semantically associated with a canonical procedure for determining whether S is true; while this procedure need not be used as a verification strategy, competent speakers are biased towards strategies that directly reflect canonical specifications of truth conditions. Evidence in favor of this hypothesis comes from a psycholinguistic experiment examining adult judgments (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • A self-organizing perceptual system.James R. Levenick - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):409-410.
  • A nonspatial solution to a spatial problem.Ronald M. Lesperance & Stephen Kaplan - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):408-409.
  • Complexity is complicated.Paul R. Kube - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):450-451.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • State transitions in constraint satisfaction networks.John K. Kruschke - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):407-408.
  • Features and locations: Dichotomy or continuum?Lester E. Krueger & Leann M. Stadtlander - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):406-407.
  • Analyzing vision at the complexity level: Misplaced complexity?Lester E. Krueger & Chiou-Yueh Tsav - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):449-450.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Experimental investigations of ambiguity: the case of most.Hadas Kotek, Yasutada Sudo & Martin Hackl - 2015 - Natural Language Semantics 23 (2):119-156.
    In the study of natural language quantification, much recent attention has been devoted to the investigation of verification procedures associated with the proportional quantifier most. The aim of these studies is to go beyond the traditional characterization of the semantics of most, which is confined to explicating its truth-functional and presuppositional content as well as its combinatorial properties, as these aspects underdetermine the correct analysis of most. The present paper contributes to this effort by presenting new experimental evidence in support (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • The development of separability in visual perception.Régine Kolinsky - 1989 - Cognition 33 (3):243-284.
  • On distinguishing epistemic from pragmatic action.David Kirsh & Paul Maglio - 1994 - Cognitive Science 18 (4):513-49.
    We present data and argument to show that in Tetris - a real-time interactive video game - certain cognitive and perceptual problems are more quickly, easily, and reliably solved by performing actions in the world rather than by performing computational actions in the head alone. We have found that some translations and rotations are best understood as using the world to improve cognition. These actions are not used to implement a plan, or to implement a reaction; they are used to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   207 citations  
  • Visual search for schematic affective faces: Stability and variability of search slopes with different instances.Gernot Horstmann - 2009 - Cognition and Emotion 23 (2):355-379.
    The threat-advantage hypothesis that threatening or negative faces can be discriminated preattentively has often been tested in the visual search paradigm with schematic stimuli. The results have been heterogeneous, suggesting that the choice of particular stimuli have profound effects on search efficiency. Because this conclusion is hampered by differences in experimental procedure, I selected examples from past literature and presented replicas of stimulus pairs (schematic positive and negative faces) in a within-participants design. Although there was a consistent advantage for angry-face (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • The feature-positive effect in pigeons: Conditionality, overall predictiveness, and type of feature.Eliot Hearst - 1988 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 26 (1):73-76.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation