Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Gombrich and the Duck-Rabbit.Robert Eamon Briscoe - 2015 - In Michael Beaney, Brendan Harrington & Dominic Shaw (eds.), Aspect Perception After Wittgenstein: Seeing-as and Novelty. New York: Routledge. pp. 49-88.
  • The validation problem.Donald M. Wilkie - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):349-350.
  • In defense of exaptation.Wendy Wilkins & Jennie Dumford - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):763-764.
  • Are there really two types of learning?Yorick Wilks - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):671-671.
  • The hard questions about noninductive learning remain unanswered.Eric Wanner - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):670-670.
  • Revisiting Arnheim and Gombrich in Social Scientific Perspective.Ian Verstegen - 2018 - Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 76 (1):45-55.
    This article revisits an earlier social scientific analysis of the thought of Rudolf Arnheim and E. H. Gombrich. Adding to the earlier analysis in terms of social ontology and historical development is an analysis of the sufficiency of perception to yield information about the world, both in ordinary and in artistic contexts. Gombrich held to an idea of perception as hypothesis testing, and it joins with Popper's philosophy in the deferred warrant of the perceptual image. Arnheim, instead, followed the Gestalt (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Explicit Learning of Arbitrary and Non-Arbitrary Action–Effect Relations in Adults and 4-Year-Olds.Stephan A. Verschoor, Rena M. Eenshuistra, Jutta Kray, Szilvia Biro & Bernhard Hommel - 2011 - Frontiers in Psychology 2.
  • Why chimps matter to language origin.Ib Ulbaek - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):762-763.
  • Toward an adaptationist psycholinguistics.John Tooby & Leda Cosmides - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):760-762.
  • Grammar yes, generative grammar no.Michael Tomasello - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):759-760.
  • Rejecting induction: Using occam's razor too soon.J. T. Tolliver - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):669-670.
  • The pragmatics of induction.Paul Thagard - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):668-669.
  • The view of language.Michael Studdert-Kennedy - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):758-759.
  • For a probabilistic sociology: A history of concept formation with Pierre Bourdieu.Michael Strand & Omar Lizardo - 2022 - Theory and Society 51 (3):399-434.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The evolution of the language faculty: A paradox and its solution.Dan Sperber - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):756-758.
  • Salvaging parts of the “classical theory” of categorization.Dan Sperber - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):668-668.
  • Genetic aspects to differences in foraging behavior.Marla B. Sokolowski - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):348-349.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Anatomizing the rhinoceros.Elliott Sober - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):764-765.
  • Black box inference: When should intervening variables be postulated?Elliott Sober - 1998 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 49 (3):469-498.
    An empirical procedure is suggested for testing a model that postulates variables that intervene between observed causes and abserved effects against a model that includes no such postulate. The procedure is applied to two experiments in psychology. One involves a conditioning regimen that leads to response generalization; the other concerns the question of whether chimpanzees have a theory of mind.
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  • Levels of explanation.Mark Snyderman - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):348-348.
  • Category differences/automaticity.Edward E. Smith - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):667-667.
  • Theory-laden concepts: Great, but what is the next step?Charles P. Shimp - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):666-667.
  • Questions about foraging.Sara J. Shettleworth - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):347-348.
  • The learning of function and the function of learning.Roger C. Schank, Gregg C. Collins & Lawrence E. Hunter - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):672-686.
  • Transcending inductive category formation in learning.Roger C. Schank, Gregg C. Collins & Lawrence E. Hunter - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):639-651.
    The inductive category formation framework, an influential set of theories of learning in psychology and artificial intelligence, is deeply flawed. In this framework a set of necessary and sufficient features is taken to define a category. Such definitions are not functionally justified, are not used by people, and are not inducible by a learning system. Inductive theories depend on having access to all and only relevant features, which is not only impossible but begs a key question in learning. The crucial (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   58 citations  
  • Is simulated foraging similar to natural foraging?Masaya Sato & Takayuki Sakagami - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):346-347.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Of rats and men.Neil Rowland - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):346-346.
  • Arbitrariness no argument against adaption.Mark Ridley - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):756-756.
  • Behaviourism, Consciousness and the Philosophy of Psychology.Edward S. Reed - 1981 - Philosophy of the Social Sciences 11 (4):477-484.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Neurath’s Congestions, Depth of Intention, and Precization: Arne Naess and His Viennese Heritage.Jan Radler - 2013 - Hopos: The Journal of the International Society for the History of Philosophy of Science 3 (1):59-90.
    In recent years, a significant amount of research has investigated the Vienna Circle’s ramifications. Otto Neurath has received much attention as one of the most prominent and energetic adherents, but less conspicuous philosophers now find themselves at the center of historical research. This article’s aim is to investigate Arne Naess’s connection to Logical Empiricism. Two crucial influences on Naess’s work are identified: Otto Neurath and the psychologist Egon Brunswik. This article’s most significant contributions are that, from the perspective of a (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the coevolution of language and social competence.David Premack - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):754-756.
  • Natural language and natural selection.Steven Pinker & Paul Bloom - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):707-27.
    Many people have argued that the evolution of the human language faculty cannot be explained by Darwinian natural selection. Chomsky and Gould have suggested that language may have evolved as the by-product of selection for other abilities or as a consequence of as-yet unknown laws of growth and form. Others have argued that a biological specialization for grammar is incompatible with every tenet of Darwinian theory – that it shows no genetic variation, could not exist in any intermediate forms, confers (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   598 citations  
  • Issues in the evolution of the human language faculty.Steven Pinker & Paul Bloom - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):765-784.
  • An ideological battle over modals and quantifiers.Massimo Piattelli-Palmarini - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):752-754.
  • Complexity and adaptation.David Pesetsky & Ned Block - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):750-752.
  • Foraging and feeding in operant simulations.Blaine F. Peden - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):345-346.
  • Approaches, assumptions, and goals in modeling cognitive behavior.Richard E. Pastore & David G. Payne - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):665-666.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The emergence of homo loquens and the laws of physics.Carlos P. Otero - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):747-750.
  • The genome might as well store the entire language in the environment.Anat Ninio - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):746-747.
  • Natural selection and the autonomy of syntax.Frederick J. Newmeyer - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):745-746.
  • The psychology of category learning: Current status and future prospect.Gregory L. Murphy - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):664-665.
  • Of what use categories?Ruth Garrett Millikan - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):663-664.
  • Outcome and mechanism in foraging.Roger L. Mellgren - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):344-345.
  • Optimal foraging for operant conditioners.James N. McNair - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):343-344.
  • Middle position on language, cognition, and evolution.Michael Maratsos - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):744-745.
  • Causal stories.David Magnus - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):744-744.
  • Adaptive complexity in sound patterns.Björn Lindblom - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):743-744.
  • Language evolved – So what's new?John Limber - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):742-743.
  • Not invented here.Philip Lieberman - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):741-742.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How much did the brain have to change for speech?R. C. Lewontin - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):740-741.