Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Even “simple” systems are more complex than we thick.Fred Delcomyn - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):544-545.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Can neuroethologists be led?Fred Delcomyn - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):385.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cerebral play of forces in offensive-defensive mechanisms.José M. R. Delgado - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):217-218.
  • Selectionist mechanisms: A framework for interactionism.Stanislas Dehaene & Jean-Pierre Changeux - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):633-633.
  • Neurotransmitter organization of aggressive behavior.László Decsi & Julia Nagy - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):216-217.
  • Motivation, decision-making, and choice.Marian Stamp Dawkins - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):134-135.
  • Burying the vehicle.Richard Dawkins - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):616-617.
  • Neuroethology: Why put it in a straitjacket?Jackson Davis - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):384.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neurophilosophical reflections on central nervous pattern generations.William J. Davis - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):543-544.
  • Expectancy bias and phobias: Accounting for the uneven distribution of fears and the characteristics of clinical phobias.Graham C. L. Davey - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (2):315-325.
  • Human ceremonial ritual and the modulation of aggression.Eugene G. D'Aquili - 1985 - Zygon 20 (1):21-30.
    . Human ceremonial ritual is considered as an evolved behavior, one of the principal effects of which is the promotion of intragroup cohesion by decreasing or eliminating intragroup aggression. It is seen as a major determinant of what Victor Turner calls communitas in human social groups of varying extension. The frequent paradoxical effect of ritual's promoting extragroup aggression at the same time that it diminishes intragroup aggression is considered. A neuroevolutionary model of the development and social effects of ritual behavior (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Too many errors.Martin Daly - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):306-307.
  • The evolution of cognition—a hypothesis.Holk Cruse - 2003 - Cognitive Science 27 (1):135-155.
    Behavior may be controlled by reactive systems. In a reactive system the motor output is exclusively driven by actual sensory input. An alternative solution to control behavior is given by “cognitive” systems capable of planning ahead. To this end the system has to be equipped with some kind of internal world model. A sensible basis of an internal world model might be a model of the system's own body. I show that a reactive system with the ability to control a (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • In praise of replicators.James F. Crow - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):616-616.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Hypothesis testing and social engineering.Lee Cronk - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):305-306.
  • Human pugnacity and war: Some anticipations of sociobiology, 1880–1919. [REVIEW]Paul Crook - 1998 - Biology and Philosophy 13 (2):263-288.
    Almost all of the themes contained in E.O.Wilson's sociobiological writing on war and human aggression were prefigured in Anglo-American bio-social discourse, c. 1880–1919. Instinct theory – stemming from animal psychology and the genetics revolution – encouraged the belief that pugnacity had been programmed into the ancient part of the human brain as a result of evolutionary pressures dating from prehistory. War was seen to be instinct-driven, and genocidal fighting postulated as a eugenic force in early human evolution. War was explained (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Group selection's new clothes.Lee Cronk - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):615-616.
  • The ethological constitution of animals as natural objects: The technical writings of Konrad Lorenz and Nikolaas Tinbergen. [REVIEW]Eileen Crist - 1998 - Biology and Philosophy 13 (1):61-102.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The reemergence of evolutionary psychology?Charles Crawford & Tracy Lindberg - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):305-305.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Oscillators in human motor systems.Brian Craske - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):621-622.
  • Naturalizing language: human appraisal and (quasi) technology.Stephen J. Cowley - 2013 - AI and Society 28 (4):443-453.
    Using contemporary science, the paper builds on Wittgenstein’s views of human language. Rather than ascribing reality to inscription-like entities, it links embodiment with distributed cognition. The verbal or (quasi) technological aspect of language is traced to not action, but human specific interactivity. This species-specific form of sense-making sustains, among other things, using texts, making/construing phonetic gestures and thinking. Human action is thus grounded in appraisals or sense-saturated coordination. To illustrate interactivity at work, the paper focuses on a case study. Over (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Social versus ecological intelligence.Marina Cords - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):151-151.
  • Striatal structures, dopamine and the mobility gradient model.Alexander R. Cools - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):271-272.
  • Sensorimotor functions: What is a command, that a code may yield it?Christopher M. Comer - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):372-372.
  • Culture: The Driving Force of Human Cognition.Ivan Colagè & Francesco D'Errico - 2018 - Topics in Cognitive Science 12 (2):654-672.
    An overview on archaeological evidence, provided by Colagè and d’Errico, reveals that the timing, location, and pace of cultural innovations are more consistent with scenarios that take culture, rather than genetic evolutionary processes, as the key driving force for human cognition. The authors elaborate on those mechanisms by which cultural evolution operates, with a specific focus on cultural exaptation and cultural neural reuse.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • A new generation of experimental and theoretical methods is needed in neuroblology.Avis H. Cohen - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):543-543.
  • A teleofunctional account of evolutionary mismatch.Nathan Cofnas - 2016 - Biology and Philosophy 31 (4):507-525.
    When the environment in which an organism lives deviates in some essential way from that to which it is adapted, this is described as “evolutionary mismatch,” or “evolutionary novelty.” The notion of mismatch plays an important role, explicitly or implicitly, in evolution-informed cognitive psychology, clinical psychology, and medicine. The evolutionary novelty of our contemporary environment is thought to have significant implications for our health and well-being. However, scientists have generally been working without a clear definition of mismatch. This paper defines (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Unnecessary competition requirement makes group selection harder to demonstrate.F. T. Cloak - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):614-615.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On natural selection and culture.F. T. Cloak - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):238-240.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The algorithm/implementation distinction.Austen Clark - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):480-480.
  • Functional principles and situated problem solving.William J. Clancey - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):479-480.
  • Difficulties and relevance of a neuroethological approach to neurobiology.F. Clarac - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):383.
  • Applications and limitations of dynamic programming in behavioral theory.Colin W. Clark - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):134-134.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Précis of How monkeys see the world.Dorothy L. Cheney & Robert M. Seyfarth - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):135-147.
  • Characterizing the mind of another species.Dorothy L. Cheney & Robert M. Seyfarth - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):172-182.
  • The diversity of variability.William D. Chapple - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):602-602.
  • Sleepwalking is out, but is dualism back in?William R. Charlesworth - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):303-304.
  • Social interaction: The missing link in evolutionary models.Ivan D. Chase - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):237-238.
  • Perceiving the Present and a Systematization of Illusions.Mark A. Changizi, Andrew Hsieh, Romi Nijhawan, Ryota Kanai & Shinsuke Shimojo - 2008 - Cognitive Science 32 (3):459-503.
    Over the history of the study of visual perception there has been great success at discovering countless visual illusions. There has been less success in organizing the overwhelming variety of illusions into empirical generalizations (much less explaining them all via a unifying theory). Here, this article shows that it is possible to systematically organize more than 50 kinds of illusion into a 7 × 4 matrix of 28 classes. In particular, this article demonstrates that (1) smaller sizes, (2) slower speeds, (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Independence and interaction in behavioral units.William Chapple - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):620-621.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • An ecological approach toward a unified theory of learning.William R. Charlesworth - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):142-143.
  • On the careful use of ecological models.Thomas Caraco - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):680-681.
  • On goals, perceptions, and self-control.Charles S. Carver - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):681-682.
  • Is the distinction between Type I and Type II behaviors related to the effects of septal lesions?Neil R. Carlson - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):479-479.
  • Fitness, currencies, and models.Thomas Caraco - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):133-133.
  • Variations of reflex parameters and their implications for the control of movements.Charles Capaday & Richard B. Stein - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):600-602.
  • Folk psychology redux.Linnda R. Caporael - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):302-303.
  • Levels of organization, selection, and information storage in biological and social evaluation.Donald T. Campbell - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):236-237.
  • How is a toad not like a bug?Jeffrey M. Camhi - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):371-372.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Ambivalently held group-optimizing predispositions.Donald T. Campbell & John B. Gatewood - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):614-614.