Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Synchronization and cognitive carpentry: From systematic structuring to simple reasoning. E. Koerner - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):465-466.
  • Adaptation and attention.Steven W. Zucker - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):458-458.
  • Only half way up.Andrew W. Young - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):558-558.
  • Ethereal oscillations.Malcolm P. Young - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):476-477.
  • Complexity, guided search, and the data.Jeremy M. Wolfe - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):457-458.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The primary visual system does not care about Previc's near-far dichotomy. Why not?Robert W. Williams - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):557-558.
  • The mystery-mastery-imagery complex.H. T. A. Whiting & R. P. Ingvaldsen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):228-229.
  • Fixations or smooth eye movements?Alexander H. Wertheim - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):281-282.
  • Connectionist learning and the challenge of real environments.Mark Weaver & Stephen Kaplan - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):510-511.
  • Potential disparities between imagining and preparing motor skills.Charles B. Walter & Stephan P. Swinnen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):227-228.
  • A brief history of connectionism and its psychological implications.S. F. Walker - 1990 - AI and Society 4 (1):17-38.
    Critics of the computational connectionism of the last decade suggest that it shares undesirable features with earlier empiricist or associationist approaches, and with behaviourist theories of learning. To assess the accuracy of this charge the works of earlier writers are examined for the presence of such features, and brief accounts of those found are given for Herbert Spencer, William James and the learning theorists Thorndike, Pavlov and Hull. The idea that cognition depends on associative connections among large networks of neurons (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Imagery needs preparation too.Stefan Vogt - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):226-227.
  • Visual stability: What is new?P. van Donkelaar & U. Windhorst - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):280-281.
  • Connectionist models learn what?Timothy van Gelder - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):509-510.
  • Action and attention.A. H. C. Van der Heijden & Bruce Bridgeman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):225-226.
  • On brains and models.William R. Uttal - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):456-457.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Some important constraints on complexity.Leonard Uhr - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):455-456.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Dynamic-binding theory is not plausible without chaotic oscillation.Ichiro Tsuda - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):475-476.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Analyzing vision at the complexity level.John K. Tsotsos - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):423-445.
    The general problem of visual search can be shown to be computationally intractable in a formal, complexity-theoretic sense, yet visual search is extensively involved in everyday perception, and biological systems manage to perform it remarkably well. Complexity level analysis may resolve this contradiction. Visual search can be reshaped into tractability through approximations and by optimizing the resources devoted to visual processing. Architectural constraints can be derived using the minimum cost principle to rule out a large class of potential solutions. The (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   154 citations  
  • A little complexity analysis goes a long way.John K. Tsotsos - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):458-469.
  • What does calibration solve?Arnold Trehub - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):279-280.
  • Search and the detection and integration of features.Anne Treisman - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):454-455.
  • Should first-order logic be neurally plausible?David S. Touretzky & Scott E. Fahlman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):474-475.
  • Advances in neural network theory.Gérard Toulouse - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):509-509.
  • Connectionist models: Too little too soon?William Timberlake - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):508-509.
  • Temporal synchrony and the speed of visual processing.Simon J. Thorpe - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):473-474.
  • The cerebellum and memory.Richard F. Thompson - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):801-802.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Problems of extension, representation, and computational irreducibility.Patrick Suppes - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):507-508.
  • There is no “point” to space.Gary W. Strong - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):279-279.
  • Separability of reference frame distinctions from motor and visual images.Gary W. Strong - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):224-225.
  • Phase logic is biologically relevant logic.Gary W. Strong - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):472-473.
  • Different regions of space or different spaces altogether: What are the dorsal/ventral systems processing?Gary W. Strong - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):556-557.
  • Algorithmic complexity analysis does not apply to behaving organisms.Gary W. Strong - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):453-454.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The translation solution plus motion suppression account for perceived stability.Arnold E. Stoper - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):278-279.
  • The representation of egocentric space in the posterior parietal cortex.J. F. Stein - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):691-700.
  • Vector code in space constancy.E. N. Sokolov - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):278-278.
  • Stability relative to what?Jeroen B. J. Smeets & Eli Brenner - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):277-278.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Do simple associations lead to systematic reasoning?Steven Sloman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):471-472.
  • How do we satisfy our goals?Paul G. Skokowski - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):224-224.
  • Canonical representations and constructive praxis: Some developmental and linguistic considerations.Chris Sinha - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):223-224.
  • Properties of neurons in the dorsal visual pathway of the monkey.Ralph M. Siegel - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):555-556.
  • Is it really that complex? After all, there are no green elephants.Ralph M. Siegel - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):453-453.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Calibration models and ecological efference mediation theory: Toward a synthesis of indirect and direct perception theories.Wayne L. Shebilske - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):276-277.
  • There is more to learning then meeth the eye.Noel E. Sharkey - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):506-507.
  • From simple associations to systematic reasoning: A connectionist representation of rules, variables, and dynamic binding using temporal synchrony.Lokendra Shastri & Venkat Ajjanagadde - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):417-51.
    Human agents draw a variety of inferences effortlessly, spontaneously, and with remarkable efficiency – as though these inferences were a reflexive response of their cognitive apparatus. Furthermore, these inferences are drawn with reference to a large body of background knowledge. This remarkable human ability seems paradoxical given the complexity of reasoning reported by researchers in artificial intelligence. It also poses a challenge for cognitive science and computational neuroscience: How can a system of simple and slow neuronlike elements represent a large (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   121 citations  
  • A step toward modeling reflexive reasoning.Lokendra Shastri & Venkat Ajjanagadde - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):477-494.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Neurophysiology of preparation, movement and imagery.Jerome N. Sanes - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):221-223.
  • Kinaesthetic illusions as tools in understanding motor imagery.J. P. Roll, J. C. Gilhodes & R. Roll - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):220-221.
  • Useful ideas for exploiting time to engineer representations.Richard Rohwer - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):471-471.
  • On the locus of visual stability.Daniel N. Robinson - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):275-276.