Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The defense motivation system: A theory of avoidance behavior.Fred A. Masterson & Mary Crawford - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):661-675.
    A motivational system approach to avoidance behavior is presented. According to this approach, a motivational state increases the probability of relevant response patterns and establishes the appropriate or “ideal” consummatory stimuli as positive reinforcers. In the case of feeding motivation, for example, hungry rats are likely to explore and gnaw, and to learn to persist in activities correlated with the reception of consummatory stimuli produced by ingestion of palatable substances. In the case of defense motivation, fearful rats are likely to (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • What and where is the unconditioned (or conditioned) stimulus in the conditioning model of neurosis?Marvin Zuckerman - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):187-188.
  • Short-latency avoidance responses.Kazimierz Zieliński - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):186-187.
  • “Prepared fears” and the theory of conditioning.Wanda Wyrwicka - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):186-186.
  • Avoidance is in the head, not the genes.Everett J. Wyers - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):685-685.
  • A critique of Eysenck's theory of neurosis.Paul T. P. Wong - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):185-186.
  • The Eysenck and the Wolpe theories of neurosis.Joseph Wolpe - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):184-185.
  • Advances toward a biological theory of aversive learning: Flirtation or commitment?Dallas Treit & Marcia L. Spetch - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):684-685.
  • A cognitive-incentive view.Frederick M. Toates - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):683-684.
  • Implications of recent research in conditioning for the conditioning model of neurosis.William S. Terry - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):183-184.
  • Conditioned alpha fear responses and protection from extinction.S. Soltysik - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):182-183.
  • Modeling neurosis: one type of learning is not enough.Kurt Salzinger - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):181-182.
  • Thesis and antithesis: S-R levers or meaning-perceivers?Ted L. Rosenthal - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):181-181.
  • Journey into the interior of the organism.Howard Rachlin - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):180-181.
  • To prevent means to know: Explicit but no implicit agency for prevention behavior.Roland Pfister, Solveig Tonn, Lisa Weller, Wilfried Kunde & Katharina A. Schwarz - 2021 - Cognition 206 (C):104489.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Given the chance, the normal brain can casually avoid what it would otherwise intensely fear.Jaak Panksepp & Larry Normansell - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):682-683.
  • Toward an unpdated model of neurosis.J. M. Notterman - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):178-179.
  • Reinforcement of avoidance behavior.Arlo K. Myers - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):681-682.
  • New perspectives on conditioning models and incubation theory.Susan Mineka - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):178-178.
  • Are the concepts of enhancement and preparedness necessary?Wallace R. McAllister & Dorothy E. McAllister - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):177-178.
  • Avoidance behavior: Assumptions, theory, and metatheory.Fred A. Masterson & Mary Crawford - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):685-696.
  • Conditioning models for clinical syndromes are out of date.Isaac Marks - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):175-177.
  • Reflections on the conditioning model of neurosi.Michael J. Mahoney - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):174-175.
  • On some key concepts in Eysenck's conditioning theory of neurosis.William Lyons - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):174-174.
  • Expecting shock.A. W. Logue - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):680-681.
  • A reconsideration of Eysenck's conditioning model of neurosis.Donald J. Levis - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):172-174.
  • Eysenck on Watson: paying lip service to lip service.Leonard Krasner - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):172-172.
  • The uses of trauma in experiment: Traumatic stress and the history of experimental neurosis, c. 1925–1975.Ulrich Koch - 2019 - Science in Context 32 (3):327-351.
    ArgumentThe article retraces the shifting conceptualizations of psychological trauma in experimental psychopathological research in the middle decades of the twentieth century in the United States. Among researchers studying so-called experimental neuroses in animal laboratories, trauma was an often-invoked category used to denote the clash of conflicting forces believed to lead to neurotic suffering. Experimental psychologists, however, soon grew skeptical of the traumatogenic model and ultimately came to reject neurosis as a disease entity. Both theoretical differences and practical circumstances, such as (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Eysenck's model of neurotigenesis.H. D. Kimmel - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):171-172.
  • On the sufficiency of a Pavlovian conditioning model for coping with the complexities of neurosis.Arne Öhman & Holger Ursin - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):179-180.
  • Avoidance theory: Old wine, older bottles, a few new labels.Philip N. Hineline - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):679-680.
  • Is there any need for conditioning in Eysenck's conditioning model of neurosis?Jeffrey A. Gray - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):169-171.
  • The Value of Ellul’s Analysis in Understanding Propaganda in the Helping Professions.Eileen Gambrill - 2012 - Bulletin of Science, Technology and Society 32 (3):187-202.
    This article draws on Ellul’s analysis of propaganda in understanding propaganda in the helping professions. Key in such an analysis is the interweaving of the psychological and sociological. Contrary to the discourse in mission statements of professional organizations and their codes of ethics calling for informed consent, competence of professionals and taking advantage of research findings, in everyday practice we find a variety of avoidable lapses, including decontextualized problem framing, bogus claims concerning risks, accuracy of assessment measures, and effectiveness of (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The conditioning theory of neurosis: criticisms considered.H. J. Eysenck - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):188-199.
  • The conditioning model of neurosis.H. J. Eysenck - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):155-166.
    The long-term persistence of neurotic symptoms, such as anxiety, poses difficult problems for any psychological theory. An attempt is made to revive the Watson-Mowrer conditioning theory and to avoid the many criticisms directed against it in the past. It is suggested that recent research has produced changes in learning theory that can be used to render this possible. In the first place, the doctrine of equipotentiality has been shown to be wrong, and some such concept as Seligman's “preparedness” is required, (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   96 citations  
  • The Gantt and Eysenck conditioning models for neurosis.Roscoe A. Dykman - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):168-169.
  • Is this defense needed?James A. Dinsmoor - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):679-679.
  • An emotion-attribution approach to moral behavior: Interfacing cognitive and avoidance theories of moral development.Richard A. Dienstbier - 1975 - Psychological Review 82 (4):299-315.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Arousal and physiological toughness: Implications for mental and physical health.Richard A. Dienstbier - 1989 - Psychological Review 96 (1):84-100.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Fear of the shock side as a function of acquisition criterion in one-way avoidance.Dennis J. Delprato - 1974 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 3 (3):166-168.
  • A theory in need of defense.Marc N. Branch - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):678-679.
  • Incubation and the relevance of functional CS exposure.T. D. Borkovec - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):168-168.
  • The motivation and/or reinforcement of avoidance behavior.Robert C. Bolles - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):677-678.
  • The nonextinction of fear: operation bootstrap.Robert C. Bolles - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):167-168.
  • Conditioning theory and neurosis.Dalbir Bindra - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):166-167.
  • Avoidance theory: Solutions or more problems?Philip J. Bersh - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):676-677.
  • Pleasure and aversion: Challenging the conventional dichotomy.George Ainslie - 2009 - Inquiry: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy 52 (4):357 – 377.
    Philosophy and its descendents in the behavioral sciences have traditionally divided incentives into those that are sought and those that are avoided. Positive incentives are held to be both attractive and memorable because of the direct effects of pleasure. Negative incentives are held to be unattractive but still memorable (the problem of pain) because they force unpleasant emotions on an individual by an unmotivated process, either a hardwired response (unconditioned response) or one substituted by association (conditioned response). Negative incentives are (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Defense motivational system: Issues of emotion, reinforcement, and neural structure.David Adams - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):675-676.
  • Manifestations and Consequences of Negative Information’s Great Diversity.Hans Alves - unknown
    In the present dissertation, I propose a general, robust, and objective characteristic of the information environment, according to which negative information is more diverse than positive information. I present an explanatory framework for this phenomenon based on the non-extremity of positive qualities. Specifically, most attribute dimensions host one “positive” range which is surrounded by two distinct “negative” ranges, resulting in a greater diversity of negative compared to positive attributes, stimuli, and information in general. Chapter 1 of my dissertation reviews evidence (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark