Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The meaning of representation in animal memory.H. L. Roitblat - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):353-372.
    A representation is a remnant of previous experience that allows that experience to affect later behavior. This paper develops a metatheoretical view of representation and applies it to issues concerning representation in animals. To describe a representational system one must specify the following: thedomainor range of situations in the represented world to which the system applies; thecontentor set of features encoded and preserved by the system; thecodeor transformational rules relating features of the representation to the corresponding features of the represented (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   312 citations  
  • Mental States Are Like Diseases.Sander Verhaegh - 2019 - In Robert Sinclair (ed.), Science and Sensibilia by W. V. Quine: The 1980 Immanuel Kant Lectures. Cham: Palgrave Macmillan.
    While Quine’s linguistic behaviorism is well-known, his Kant Lectures contain one of his most detailed discussions of behaviorism in psychology and the philosophy of mind. Quine clarifies the nature of his psychological commitments by arguing for a modest view that is against ‘excessively restrictive’ variants of behaviorism while maintaining ‘a good measure of behaviorist discipline…to keep [our mental] terms under control’. In this paper, I use Quine’s Kant Lectures to reconstruct his position. I distinguish three types of behaviorism in psychology (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What's the stimulus?G. E. Zuriff - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):664-664.
  • Précis of Behaviorism: A conceptual reconstruction.G. E. Zuriff - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):687-699.
    The conceptual framework of behaviorism is reconstructed in a logical scheme rather than along chronological lines. The resulting reconstruction is faithful to the history of behaviorism and yet meets the contemporary challenges arising from cognitive science, psycholinguistics, and philosophy. In this reconstruction, the fundamental premise is that psychology is to be a natural science, and the major corollaries are that psychology is to be objective and empirical. To a great extent, the reconstruction of behaviorism is an elaboration of behaviorist views (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Conceptual reconstruction: A reconstruction.G. E. Zuriff - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):716-723.
  • The assessment of intentionality in animals.Thomas R. Zentall - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):663-663.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The cost of an interrupted response pattern.Thomas R. Zentall - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):147-148.
  • The heuristic value of representation.Thomas R. Zentall - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):393-394.
  • The return of the reinforcement theorists.C. D. L. Wynne - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):156-156.
  • Some epistemological and methodological issues in clinical research.Benjamin B. Wolman - 1966 - Inquiry: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy 9 (1-4):171 – 184.
    Epistemological realism was postulated as a prolegomenon to clinical research. Observation of single cases must precede any effort for generalization. Observation of men by men is always a field process. In clinical research the experimenter exercises a great amount of power over the subject, thus a naive empirical approach and operationism may be misleading. Clinical theory must be coated in a language different from empirical data and enable the formation of causal chains of events.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the creation of classification systems of memory.Daniel B. Willingham - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):426-427.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Mind the brain.Martha Wilson - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):393-393.
  • A mathematical theory of reinforcement: An unexpected place to find support for analogical memory coding.Donald M. Wilkie & Lisa M. Saksida - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):155-156.
  • Commitment: Beyond Rachlin's control?N. E. Wetherick - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):146-147.
    Rachlin's view of self-control is rejected on the grounds that his arguments do not establish the possibility of abstract, external, stimulus patterns and that his experiments, although they show that pigeons and human beings do sometimes choose postponed rather than immediate gratification, do not challenge the commonly held view that internal factors are involved in the former choice.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Fifty years on: The new “principles of behavior”?J. H. Wearden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):155-155.
  • Pattern proliferation in teleological behaviorism.Bruce N. Waller - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):145-146.
  • Not “pain and behavior” but pain in behavior.Patrick D. Wall - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):73-73.
  • How general is a general theory of reinforcement?Stephen F. Walker - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):154-155.
  • Internal representations and indeterminacy: A skeptical view.William R. Uttal - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):392-393.
  • The reign of pain fails mainly in the brain.Dennis C. Turk & Peter Salovey - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):72-73.
  • Maps of desire: Edward Tolman's drive theory of wants.Simon Torracinta - 2023 - History of the Human Sciences 36 (1):3-30.
    Wants and desires are central to ordinary experience and to aesthetic, philosophical, and theological thought. Yet despite a burgeoning interest in the history of emotions research, their history as objects of scientific study has received little attention. This historiographical neglect mirrors a real one, with the retreat of introspection in the positivist human sciences of the early 20th century culminating in the relative marginalization of questions of psychic interiority. This article therefore seeks to explain an apparent paradox: the attempt to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Further choices for molar theory.François Tonneau - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):145-145.
    The target article extends molar behaviorism in two positive ways: beyond average aggregates and beyond restricted laws of Although a molar framework based on purely overt events shows promise for advancing behavior theory, Rachlin's specific form of teleological behaviorism is in need of clarification.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Animal-centered models of reinforcement.William Timberlake - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):153-154.
  • Are some mental states public events?Nicholas S. Thompson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):662-663.
  • Animal versus human minds.H. S. Terrace - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):391-392.
  • Are infants human?H. S. Terrace - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):425-426.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Models of Cognition and Their Applications in Behavioral Economics: A Conceptual Framework for Nudging Derived From Behavior Analysis and Relational Frame Theory.Marco Tagliabue, Valeria Squatrito & Giovambattista Presti - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10:484958.
    This study puts forward a rounder conceptual model for interpreting short and long-term effects of choice behavior. Kahneman’s (2011) distinction between cognitive processing System 1 and System 2 reflect the more rigorous distinction between Brief and Immediate and Extended and Elaborated Relational Responding. Specifically, we provide theoretical accounts and applied examples of how nudging, or the manipulation of environmental contingencies, works on the creation and modification of relational frames. The subset denominated educational nudges, or boosts, are particularly useful towards their (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Short-term memory in human operant conditioning.Frode Svartdal - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):152-153.
  • The ABC of academic procrastination: Functional analysis of a detrimental habit.Frode Svartdal & Jon Arne Løkke - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Academic procrastination – habitually delaying work with academic tasks to the extent that the delays become detrimental to performance, wellbeing, and health – represents a substantial personal, systemic, and societal problem. Still, efforts to prevent and reduce it are surprisingly scarce and often offered as treatment regimens rather than preventive efforts. Based on the principles of functional analysis and a broad examination of factors that are important for academic procrastinatory behaviors, this paper aims to describe a strategy for analyzing individual (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is awareness necessary for operant conditioning?Frode Svartdal - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):424-425.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The role of discounting in global social issues.Craig Summers - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):144-144.
    The willingness to trade off large but ill-defined future consequences for immediate work characterizes social problems such as environmental sustainability. This commentary argues that important applications of behavioral models of self-control are being overlooked in the experimental literature. Tying the experimental literature to longterm health, environmental, and other risks makes the experimental work more germane, and raises new research questions for experimental modeling.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cross-fertilization between research on interpersonal communication and drug discrimination.I. P. Stolerman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):661-662.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The intellectual origins of Rational Psychotherapy.Arthur Still & Windy Dryden - 1998 - History of the Human Sciences 11 (3):63-86.
    In this paper we attempt to understand the intellectual origins of Albert Ellis' Rational Psychotherapy (now known as Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy). In his therapeutic practice Ellis used a 'lumper' argument to replace the focus of change in psychoanalysis: not the lengthy uncovering and reworking of the individual's personal history, but the demands in self-talk through which the client is currently dis turbed. In constructing around this the persuasive (rhetorical) package that became his therapy, Ellis drew on a number of (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The scale of nature: Fitted parameters and dimensional correctness.D. W. Stephens - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):150-152.
  • Whither learning, whither memory?Michael A. Stadler & Peter A. Frensch - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):423-424.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Unificationism: Philosophy for the modern disunified science of psychology.Arthur W. Staats - 1989 - Philosophical Psychology 2 (2):143-164.
    Abstract Psychology's goal has been to become a science, taking the modern natural sciences as the model. It has not been understood that each science undergoes a transition from early disunification to later unification, that a fundmental dimension is involved that differentiates sciences. Psychology is a modern disunified science, distinguished by its chaotic knowledge and ways of operating. A philosophy of science based on modem unified science, as philosophies generally are, is inappropriate as a means of understanding psychology or of (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • In the beginning was the word.J. E. R. Staddon - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):390-391.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Dissociable learning and memory systems of the brain.Larry R. Squire, Stephan Hamann & Barbara Knowlton - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):422-423.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Alternatives to radical behaviorism.Terry L. Smith - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):143-144.
    Operant psychologists are looking for alternatives to radical behaviorism. Rachlin offers teleological behaviorism, but it may pose as many difficulties as radical behaviorism. There is, however, a less drastic way to defend Rachlin's thesis of It portrays operant principles as relating distal efficient causes to behavioral effects.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is it behaviorism?B. F. Skinner - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):716-716.
  • Teleological behaviorism and internal control of behavior.Albert Silverstein - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):142-143.
  • Practical effects of response specification.Richard L. Shull - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):150-150.
  • “Suspicion,” “fear,” “contamination,” “great dangers,” and behavioral fictions.Charles P. Shimp - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):715-716.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Molar behaviorism, positivism, and pain.Charles P. Shimp - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):71-72.
  • Historicism, behaviorism, and the conceptual status of memory representations in animals.Charles P. Shimp - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):389-390.
  • Distinguishing between acts and patterns.Eliot Shimoff - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):142-142.
    The costliness of disrupting a pattern may not be a useful criterion for distinguishing between acts and patterns; there are instances in which omitted components of patterns are hard to detect (e.g., typographical errors), or in which distortions are easily introduced (e.g., slurred words in a trite phrase). Are there behavioral criteria for distinguishing between acts and patterns?
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Awareness and reinforcement.Charles P. Shimp - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):149-150.
  • Expectancy: The endogenous source of anticipatory activities, including “pseudoconditioned” responses.Patrick J. Sheafor - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):387-389.
  • Pain without behavior: Inhibition of reactions to sensation.Kelly G. Shaver & Jana J. Herrman - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):71-71.
  • How should implicit learning be characterized?David R. Shanks & Mark F. St John - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):427-447.