Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. On the what_ and _how of learning.R. C. Gonzalez & Matthew Yarczower - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):145-145.
  • Contrasting approaches to a theory of learning.Timothy D. Johnston - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):125-139.
    The general process view of learning, which guided research into learning for the first half of this century, has come under attack in recent years from several quarters. One form of criticism has come from proponents of the so-called biological boundaries approach to learning. These theorists have presented a variety of data showing that supposedly general laws of learning may in fact be limited in their applicability to different species and learning tasks, and they argue that the limitations are drawn (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   287 citations  
  • Missing variables in studies of animal learning.Wally Welker - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):161-161.
  • Clinical artificial intelligence.Virginia Teller & Hartvig Dahl - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):549-550.
  • Is PARRY paranoid?David W. Swanson - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):548-549.
  • The sciences of the artificial.Mark Stefik - 1984 - Artificial Intelligence 22 (1):95-97.
  • Testability of expert systems in system development and application.Elke Steven, Michael Hoenen & Matthias Kloth - 1992 - AI and Society 6 (4):337-344.
    In this paper the difficulties arising out of a necessary examination of expert systems as to the ‘correctness’ of functioning are outlined. The argumentation is based on the problematic use of the knowledge term in expert system development and the design perspectives connected with the cognitivistic knowledge concept. It becomes obvious that fundamental problems in system development will involve negative consequences for utilization. The perspective developed from this analysis is assuming that these problems have to be taken into account in (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Deep and shallow simulations.Aaron Sloman - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):548-548.
  • An ecological theory of learning: Good goal, poor strategy.Sara J. Shettleworth - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):160-161.
  • Ethical robots: the future can heed us. [REVIEW]Selmer Bringsjord - 2008 - AI and Society 22 (4):539-550.
    Bill Joy’s deep pessimism is now famous. Why the Future Doesn’t Need Us, his defense of that pessimism, has been read by, it seems, everyone—and many of these readers, apparently, have been converted to the dark side, or rather more accurately, to the future-is-dark side. Fortunately (for us; unfortunately for Joy), the defense, at least the part of it that pertains to AI and robotics, fails. Ours may be a dark future, but we cannot know that on the basis of (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • The ecology of learning: The right answer to the wrong question.Barry Schwartz - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):159-160.
  • Adaptive modification of behavior: Processing information from the environment.Wolfgang M. Schleidt - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):158-159.
  • Explaining diversity and searching for general processes: Isn't there a middle ground?Paul Rozin - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):157-158.
  • The overlooked ubiquity of first-person experience in the cognitive sciences.Joana Rigato, Scott M. Rennie & Zachary F. Mainen - 2019 - Synthese 198 (9):8005-8041.
    Science aims to transform the subjectivity of individual observations and ideas into more objective and universal knowledge. Yet if there is any area in which first-person experience holds a particularly special and delicate role, it is the sciences of the mind. According to a widespread view, first-person methods were largely discarded from psychology after the fall of introspectionism a century ago and replaced by more objective behavioral measures, a step that some authors have begun to criticize. To examine whether these (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • STS, Meet Data Science, Once Again.David Ribes - 2019 - Science, Technology, and Human Values 44 (3):514-539.
    Science and technology studies and the emerging field of data science share surprising elective affinities. At the growing intersections of these fields, there will be many opportunities and not a few thorny difficulties for STS scholars. First, I discuss how both fields frame the rollout of data science as a simultaneously social and technical endeavor, even if in distinct ways and for diverging purposes. Second, I discuss the logic of domains in contemporary computer, information, and data science circles. While STS (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Known general principles of learning cannot be ignored.Sam Revusky - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):156-157.
  • Evaluation of a model's test.Russell Revlin - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):547-548.
  • Psychiatry and computers: An uneasy synthesis.William H. Reid & John F. Riedler - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):547-547.
  • Learning theory in its niche.Howard Rachlin - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):155-156.
  • Is an ecological approach radical enough?H. C. Plotkin & F. J. Odling-Smee - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):154-155.
  • The dichotomous predicament of contemporary psychology.V. Pinkava - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):546-547.
  • A functional view of learning.Lewis Petrinovich - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):153-154.
  • Modeling paranoia: The cargo cult metaphor.Keith Oatley - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):545-546.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cockpit cognition: Education, the military and cognitive engineering. [REVIEW]Douglas D. Noble - 1989 - AI and Society 3 (4):271-296.
    The goals of public education, as well as conceptions of human intelligence and learning, are undergoing a transformation through the application of military-sponsored information technologies and information processing models of human thought. Recent emphases in education on thinking skills, learning strategies, and computer-based technologies are the latest episodes in the postwar military agenda to engineer intelligent components, human and artificial, for the optimal performance of complex technological systems. Public education serves increasingly as a “human factors” laboratory and production site for (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The knowledge level.Allen Newell - 1982 - Artificial Intelligence 18 (1):81-132.
  • Learning theory: Behavioral artifacts or general principles?John A. Nevin - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):152-153.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Al and cargo cult science.James Moor - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):544-545.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • PARRY and the evaluation of cognitive models.James R. Miller - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):543-544.
  • Le tournant cognitif en sémiotique.Jean-Guy Meunier - 1991 - Horizons Philosophiques 1 (2):51-80.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A fourth approach to the study of learning: Are “processes” really necessary?John C. Malone - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):151-152.
  • Testing the components of a computer model.Brendan A. Maher - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):543-543.
  • Colby's model for paranoia: It's made well, but what is it?Peter A. Magaro & Harvey G. Shulman - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):542-543.
  • Species differences and principles of learning: Informed generality.A. W. Logue - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):150-151.
  • How smart must you be to be crazy?Robert Lindsay - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):541-542.
  • An active symbols theory of chess intuition.Alexandre Linhares - 2005 - Minds and Machines 15 (2):131-181.
    The well-known game of chess has traditionally been modeled in artificial intelligence studies by search engines with advanced pruning techniques. The models were thus centered on an inference engine manipulating passive symbols in the form of tokens. It is beyond doubt, however, that human players do not carry out such processes. Instead, chess masters instead carry out perceptual processes, carefully categorizing the chunks perceived in a position and gradually building complex dynamic structures to represent the subtle pressures embedded in the (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • General process theory, ecology, and animal-human continuity: A cognitive perspective.Janet L. Lachman & Roy Lachman - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):149-150.
  • The ecological approach to learning.John Kruse & Edward Reed - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):148-149.
  • On the generality of PARRY, Colby's paranoia model.Manfred Kochen - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):540-541.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Ecology and learning.Alan C. Kamil - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):147-148.
  • A funny thing happened on the way to comparative psychology.James W. Kalat - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):147-147.
  • Expertise and Error in Diagnostic Reasoning.Paul E. Johnson, Alica S. Duran, Frank Hassebrock, James Moller, Michael Prietula, Paul J. Feltovich & David B. Swanson - 1981 - Cognitive Science 5 (3):235-283.
    An investigation is presented in which a computer simulation model (DIAGNOSER) is used to develop and test predictions for behavior of subjects in a task of medical diagnosis. The first experiment employed a process‐tracing methodology in order to compare hypothesis generation and evaluation behavior of DIAGNOSER with individuals at different levels of expertise (students, trainees, experts). A second experiment performed with only DIAGNOSER identified conditions under which errors in reasoning in the first experiment could be related to interpretation of specific (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • An ecological approach to a theory of learning.Timothy D. Johnston - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):162-173.
  • Colby's paranoia model: An old theory in a new frame?C. E. Izard & F. A. Masterson - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):539-540.
  • Biological approaches to the study of learning: Does Johnston provide a new alternative?Robert A. Hinde - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):146-147.
  • Like the breathability of air: Embodied embedded communication.Willem F. G. Haselager - 2012 - Pragmatics and Cognition 20 (2):263-274.
    I present experimental and computational research, inspired by the perspective of Embodied Embedded Cognition, concerning various aspects of language as supporting Everett's interactionist view of language. Based on earlier and ongoing work, I briefly illustrate the contribution of the environment to the systematicity displayed in linguistic performance, the importance of joint attention for the development of a shared vocabulary, the role of (limited) traveling for language diversification, the function of perspective taking in social communication, and the bodily nature of understanding (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Discussing learning: The quandary of substance.Jack P. Hailman - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):146-146.
  • Paranoia concerning program-resistant aspects of the mind - and let's drop rocks on Turing's toes again.Keith Gunderson - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):537-539.
  • The relevance of phylogenetics to the study of behavioral diversity.Michael T. Ghiselin - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):144-145.
  • The nature of learning explanations.John Garcia - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):143-144.
  • A Generative View of Rationality and Growing Awareness†.Teppo Felin & Jan Koenderink - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    In this paper we contrast bounded and ecological rationality with a proposed alternative, generative rationality. Ecological approaches to rationality build on the idea of humans as “intuitive statisticians” while we argue for a more generative conception of humans as “probing organisms.” We first highlight how ecological rationality’s focus on cues and statistics is problematic for two reasons: the problem of cue salience, and the problem of cue uncertainty. We highlight these problems by revisiting the statistical and cue-based logic that underlies (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark