Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Against semantic preprocessing in parafoveal vision.Keith Rayner - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):46-47.
  • A two-dimensional array of models of cognitive function.Gardner C. Quarton - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):48-48.
  • Subsymbols aren't much good outside of a symbol-processing architecture.Alan Prince & Steven Pinker - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):46-47.
  • Local and distributed processes in attentional orienting.Michael I. Posner - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):78-79.
  • Only the simplest dual-route theories are unreasonable.Alexander Pollatsek - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):722-723.
  • Parallel distributed processing challenges the strong modularity hypothesis, not the locality assumption.David C. Plaut - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):77-78.
  • Some reasons to save the grapheme and the phoneme.Charles A. Perfetti - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):721-722.
  • Perceptual uncertainty is a property of the cognitive system.Manuel Perea & Manuel Carreiras - 2012 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 (5):298-299.
    We qualify Frost's proposals regarding letter-position coding in visual word recognition and the universal model of reading. First, we show that perceptual uncertainty regarding letter position is not tied to European languages – instead it is a general property of the cognitive system. Second, we argue that a universal model of reading should incorporate a developmental view of the reading process.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Fusion, propagation, and structuring in belief networks.Judea Pearl - 1986 - Artificial Intelligence 29 (3):241-288.
  • The pitfalls of selective attention.Karalyn Patterson - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):721-721.
  • Dual-route theory and the consistency effect.Alan J. Parkin - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):720-721.
  • Context-sensitive inference, modularity, and the assumption of formal processing.Mitch Parsell - 2005 - Philosophical Psychology 18 (1):45-58.
    Performance on the Wason selection task varies with content. This has been taken to demonstrate that there are different cognitive modules for dealing with different conceptual domains. This implication is only legitimate if our underlying cognitive architecture is formal. A non-formal system can explain content-sensitive inference without appeal to independent inferential modules.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Book review. [REVIEW]Mitch Parsell - 2005 - Minds and Machines 15 (3-4):445-451.
  • The pilfering of awareness and guilt by association.Kenneth R. Paap - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):45-46.
  • Mis-representations.J. Bruce Overmier - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):156-157.
  • Segmentation in models of reading.Richard K. Olson & Janice M. Keenan - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):719-720.
  • Computational levels again.Mike Oaksford - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):76-77.
  • Processing of the unattended message during selective dichotic listening.R. Näätänen - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):43-44.
  • The Bayesian reader: Explaining word recognition as an optimal Bayesian decision process.Dennis Norris - 2006 - Psychological Review 113 (2):327-357.
  • So the “strong” theory loses. But are there any winners?Dennis Norris - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):718-719.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Shortlist: a connectionist model of continuous speech recognition.Dennis Norris - 1994 - Cognition 52 (3):189-234.
  • Approaches to the study of intelligence.Donald A. Norman - 1991 - Artificial Intelligence 47 (1-3):327-346.
  • Connections among connections.R. J. Nelson - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):45-46.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • What do you mean by conscious?John Morton - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):43-43.
  • In defence of neurons.Chris Mortensen - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):44-45.
  • Criticising dual-route theory: Missing the point.John Morton - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):718-718.
  • Cerebro-cerebellar learning loops and language skills.John W. Moore - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):156-156.
  • Access to the lexicon: Are there three routes?D. C. Mitchell - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):717-718.
  • A competence framework for artificial intelligence research.Lisa Miracchi - 2019 - Philosophical Psychology 32 (5):588-633.
    ABSTRACTWhile over the last few decades AI research has largely focused on building tools and applications, recent technological developments have prompted a resurgence of interest in building a genuinely intelligent artificial agent – one that has a mind in the same sense that humans and animals do. In this paper, I offer a theoretical and methodological framework for this project of investigating “artificial minded intelligence” that can help to unify existing approaches and provide new avenues for research. I first outline (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Classical conditioning: The new hyperbole.Ralph R. Miller - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):155-156.
  • Distributed locality and large-scale neurocognitive networks.M. Marsel Mesulam - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):74-76.
  • Consciousness is a “subjective” state.Philip M. Merikle & Jim Cheesman - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):42-42.
  • The Place of Modeling in Cognitive Science.James L. McClelland - 2009 - Topics in Cognitive Science 1 (1):11-38.
    I consider the role of cognitive modeling in cognitive science. Modeling, and the computers that enable it, are central to the field, but the role of modeling is often misunderstood. Models are not intended to capture fully the processes they attempt to elucidate. Rather, they are explorations of ideas about the nature of cognitive processes. In these explorations, simplification is essential—through simplification, the implications of the central ideas become more transparent. This is not to say that simplification has no downsides; (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • Semantic activation and reading.George W. McConkie - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):41-42.
  • Putting knowledge in its place: A scheme for programming parallel processing structures on the fly.James L. McClelland - 1985 - Cognitive Science 9 (1):113-146.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   56 citations  
  • Neuropsychology: Going loco?Rosaleen A. McCarthy - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):73-74.
  • Interactive Activation and Mutual Constraint Satisfaction in Perception and Cognition.James L. McClelland, Daniel Mirman, Donald J. Bolger & Pranav Khaitan - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (6):1139-1189.
    In a seminal 1977 article, Rumelhart argued that perception required the simultaneous use of multiple sources of information, allowing perceivers to optimally interpret sensory information at many levels of representation in real time as information arrives. Building on Rumelhart's arguments, we present the Interactive Activation hypothesis—the idea that the mechanism used in perception and comprehension to achieve these feats exploits an interactive activation process implemented through the bidirectional propagation of activation among simple processing units. We then examine the interactive activation (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Epistemological challenges for connectionism.John McCarthy - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):44-44.
  • Distributed memory and the representation of general and specific information.James L. McClelland & David E. Rumelhart - 1985 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 114 (2):159-188.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   185 citations  
  • The Processing of Visual and Phonological Configurations of Chinese One- and Two-Character Words in a Priming Task of Semantic Categorization.Bosen Ma, Xiaoyun Wang & Degao Li - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition.William D. Marslen-Wilson - 1987 - Cognition 25 (1-2):71-102.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   126 citations  
  • Consciousness and processing: Choosing and testing a null hypothesis.Anthony J. Marcel - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):40-41.
  • The psychophysics of subliminal perception.Neil A. Macmillan - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):38-39.
  • Contiguity, contingency, adaptiveness, and controls.Glenda MacQueen, James MacRae & Shepard Siegel - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):154-155.
  • Symbols, subsymbols, neurons.William G. Lycan - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):43-44.
  • Conscious identification: Where do you draw the line?Stephen J. Lupker - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):37-38.
  • Ockham's razor at work: Modeling of the ``homunculus''. [REVIEW]András Lörincz, Barnabás Póczos, Gábor Szirtes & Bálint Takács - 2002 - Brain and Mind 3 (2):187-220.
    There is a broad consensus about the fundamental role of thehippocampal system (hippocampus and its adjacent areas) in theencoding and retrieval of episodic memories. This paper presents afunctional model of this system. Although memory is not asingle-unit cognitive function, we took the view that the wholesystem of the smooth, interrelated memory processes may have acommon basis. That is why we follow the Ockham's razor principleand minimize the size or complexity of our model assumption set.The fundamental assumption is the requirement of (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The dark side of hegemony.Charles Locurto - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):153-154.
  • Extending the “new hegemony” of classical conditioning.Dan Lloyd - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):152-153.