Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Connectionism in the golden age of cognitive science.Dan Lloyd - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):42-43.
  • Can this treatment raise the dead?Robert K. Lindsay - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):41-42.
  • Do we look for independence or near decomposability?Alan Lesgold & Kathleen L. Hammond - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):716-717.
  • Physics, cognition, and connectionism: An interdisciplinary alchemy.Wendy G. Lehnert - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):40-41.
  • Approaches to consciousness: Psychophysics or philosophy?Richard Latto & John Campion - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):36-37.
  • Smolensky, semantics, and the sensorimotor system.George Lakoff - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):39-40.
  • The relation between linguistic structure and associative theories of language learning.Joel Lachter & Thomas G. Bever - 1988 - Cognition 28 (1-2):195-247.
  • The relation between linguistic structure and associative theories of language learning—A constructive critique of some connectionist learning models.Joel Lachter & Thomas G. Bever - 1988 - Cognition 28 (1-2):195-247.
  • Classical conditioning beyond the laboratory.Hugh Lacey - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):152-152.
  • Pavlovian conditioning: Providing a bridge between cognition and biology.Marvin D. Krank - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):151-151.
  • Beyond respondent conditioning.Sibylle Klosterhalfen & Wolfgang Klosterhalfen - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):149-150.
  • A promising new strategy for studying conditioned Immunomodulation.Wolfgang Klosterhalfen - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):150-150.
  • Do neuropsychologists think in terms of interactive models?Marcel Kinsbourne - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):72-73.
  • The importance of classical conditioning.H. D. Kimmel - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):148-149.
  • Complexity at the organismic and neuronal levels.R. W. Kentridge - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):147-148.
  • Associative theory versus classical conditioning: Their proper relationship.E. James Kehoe - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):147-147.
  • Size and salience of spelling-sound correspondences.Janice Kay - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):715-716.
  • A hybrid architecture for working memory: Reply to MacDonald and Christiansen (2002).Marcel Adam Just & Sashank Varma - 2002 - Psychological Review 109 (1):55-65.
  • Perceptual units in word recognition.James F. Juola - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):715-715.
  • Semantic activation, consciousness, and attention.William A. Johnston - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):35-36.
  • What is classical conditioning?W. J. Jacobs - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):146-146.
  • Attentional orienting precedes conscious identification.Albrecht Werner Inhoff - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):35-35.
  • Phonological effects in the visual processing of words: Some methodological considerations.Albrecht Werner Inhoff - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):714-715.
  • Some memory, but no mind.Lawrence E. Hunter - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):37-38.
  • Visual word processing: Procedures, representations, and routes.Glyn W. Humphreys & Lindsay J. Evett - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):728-739.
  • 2. Information Processing Systems Which Embody Computational Rules: The Connectionist Approach.Glyn W. Humphreys - 1986 - Mind and Language 1 (3):201-12.
  • Go with the flow but mind the details.Glyn W. Humphreys & M. Jane Riddoch - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):71-72.
  • Are there independent lexical and nonlexical routes in word processing? An evaluation of the dual-route theory of reading.Glyn W. Humphreys & Lindsay J. Evett - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):689-705.
  • Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal.Daniel Holender - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):1-23.
    When the stored representation of the meaning of a stimulus is accessed through the processing of a sensory input it is maintained in an activated state for a certain amount of time that allows for further processing. This semantic activation is generally accompanied by conscious identification, which can be demonstrated by the ability of a person to perform discriminations on the basis of the meaning of the stimulus. The idea that a sensory input can give rise to semantic activation without (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   447 citations  
  • Preparatory response hypotheses: A muddle of causal and functional analyses.Karen L. Hollis - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):145-146.
  • Conceptual, experimental, and theoretical indeterminacies in research on semantic activation without conscious identification.Daniel Holender - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):50-66.
  • Common sense and conceptual halos.Douglas R. Hofstadter - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):35-37.
  • Oral reading: Duel but not rout.Leslie Henderson - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):713-714.
  • On the obvious treatment of connectionism.Stephen José Hanson - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):38-39.
  • No threat to modularity.Yosef Grodzinsky & Uri Hadar - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):70-71.
  • Neural Facades: Visual Representations of Static and Moving Form‐And‐Color‐And‐Depth.Stephen Grossberg - 1990 - Mind and Language 5 (4):411-456.
  • Does Perceptual Consciousness Overflow Cognitive Access? The Challenge from Probabilistic, Hierarchical Processes.Steven Gross & Jonathan Flombaum - 2017 - Mind and Language 32 (3):358-391.
    Does perceptual consciousness require cognitive access? Ned Block argues that it does not. Central to his case are visual memory experiments that employ post-stimulus cueing—in particular, Sperling's classic partial report studies, change-detection work by Lamme and colleagues, and a recent paper by Bronfman and colleagues that exploits our perception of ‘gist’ properties. We argue contra Block that these experiments do not support his claim. Our reinterpretations differ from previous critics' in challenging as well a longstanding and common view of visual (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Competitive Learning: From Interactive Activation to Adaptive Resonance.Stephen Grossberg - 1987 - Cognitive Science 11 (1):23-63.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   245 citations  
  • Classical conditioning: The role of interdisciplinary theory.Stephen Grossberg - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):144-145.
  • Statistical rationality.Richard M. Golden - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):35-35.
  • A Developmental Neural Model of Visual Word Perception.Richard M. Golden - 1986 - Cognitive Science 10 (3):241-276.
    A neurally plausible model of how the process of visually perceiving a letter in the context of a word is learned, and how such processing occurs in adults is proposed. The model consists of a collection of abstract letter feature detector neurons and their interconnections. The model also includes a learning rule that specifies how these interconnections evolve with experience. The interconnections between neurons can be interpreted as representing the spatially redundant, sequentially redundant, and transgraphemic information in letter string displays. (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Clarifying the locality assumption.Clark Glymour - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):69-70.
  • Further complications for dual-route theory.Robert J. Glushko - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):712-713.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Beyond Pavlovian classical conditioning.Beatrix T. Gardner & R. Allen Gardner - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):143-144.
  • Flights of teleological fancy about classical conditioning do not produce valid science or useful technology.John J. Furedy - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):142-143.
  • Towards a universal model of reading.Ram Frost, Christina Behme, Madeleine El Beveridge, Thomas H. Bak, Jeffrey S. Bowers, Max Coltheart, Stephen Crain, Colin J. Davis, S. Hélène Deacon & Laurie Beth Feldman - 2012 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 (5):263.
    In the last decade, reading research has seen a paradigmatic shift. A new wave of computational models of orthographic processing that offer various forms of noisy position or context-sensitive coding have revolutionized the field of visual word recognition. The influx of such models stems mainly from consistent findings, coming mostly from European languages, regarding an apparent insensitivity of skilled readers to letter order. Underlying the current revolution is the theoretical assumption that the insensitivity of readers to letter order reflects the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • Dynamic systems and the “subsymbolic level”.Walter J. Freeman - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):33-34.
  • Connectionism and the study of language.R. Freidin - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):34-35.
  • An operational definition of conscious awareness must be responsible to subjective experience.Carol A. Fowler - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):33-35.
  • The mechanisms of naming.K. I. Forster - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):711-712.