Switch to: References

Citations of:

Cognitive Ethology: The Minds of Other Animals

(ed.)
Lawrence Erlbaum (1991)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Perceptual representations: a teleosemantic answer to the breadth-of-application problem.Peter Https://Orcidorg288X Schulte - 2015 - Biology and Philosophy 30 (1):119-136.
    Teleosemantic theories of representation are often criticized as being “too liberal”, i.e. as categorizing states as representations which are not representational at all. Recently, a powerful version of this objection has been put forth by Tyler Burge. Focusing on perception, Burge defends the claim that all teleosemantic theories apply too broadly, thereby missing what is distinctive about representation. Contra Burge, I will argue in this paper that there is a teleosemantic account of perceptual states that does not fall prey to (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  • The question of animal culture.Bennett G. Galef - 1992 - Human Nature 3 (2):157-178.
    In this paper I consider whether traditional behaviors of animals, like traditions of humans, are transmitted by imitation learning. Review of the literature on problem solving by captive primates, and detailed consideration of two widely cited instances of purported learning by imitation and of culture in free-living primates (sweet-potato washing by Japanese macaques and termite fishing by chimpanzees), suggests that nonhuman primates do not learn to solve problems by imitation. It may, therefore, be misleading to treat animal traditions and human (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   88 citations  
  • External representation: An issue for cognition.Jiajie Zhang - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):774-775.
  • Archaeological evidence for mimetic mind and culture.Thomas Wynn - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):774-774.
  • Stages versus continuity.Christopher Wills - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):773-773.
  • Can a Saussurian ape be endowed with episodic memory only?Jacques Vauclair & Joël Fagot - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):772-773.
  • It's imitation, not mimesis.Michael Tomasello - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):771-772.
  • Language, thought and consciousness in the modern mind.Evan Thompson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):770-771.
  • Memory, text and the Greek Revolution.Jocelyn Penny Small - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):769-770.
  • How should implicit learning be characterized?David R. Shanks & Mark F. St John - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):427-447.
  • Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems.David R. Shanks & Mark F. St John - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):367-447.
    A number of ways of taxonomizing human learning have been proposed. We examine the evidence for one such proposal, namely, that there exist independent explicit and implicit learning systems. This combines two further distinctions, (1) between learning that takes place with versus without concurrent awareness, and (2) between learning that involves the encoding of instances (or fragments) versus the induction of abstract rules or hypotheses. Implicit learning is assumed to involve unconscious rule learning. We examine the evidence for implicit learning (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   192 citations  
  • Learning strategies and situated knowledge.Antonio Rizzo & Oronzo Parlangeli - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):420-421.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cognitive ethology, over-attribution of agency and focusing abilities as they relate to the origin of concepts.Carolyn A. Ristau - 2011 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34 (3):146-147.
    Carey's superb discussion of the origin of concepts is extended into the field of cognitive ethology. I also suggest that agency may be a default mechanism, often leading to over-attribution. The problem therefore becomes one of specifying the conditions in which agency is not attributed. The significance of attentional/focusing abilities on conceptual development is also emphasized.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Directed action and animal communication.Daisie Radner - 1993 - Ration 6 (2):135-54.
    Human action theory, with its emphasis on intentions and reasons, does little to enhance our understanding of the actions of nonhuman animals. Many animal (and human) actions are directed to objects in the world, including other animals. The notion of directedness can be analysed without attributing intentions or reasons to the agent. An action is directed to object X if and only if: (1) the agent singles out X, either by orientation or by selective performance of the action in the (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Directed Action and Animal Communication.Daisie Radner - 1993 - Ratio 6 (2):135-154.
    Human action theory, with its emphasis on intentions and reasons, does little to enhance our understanding of the actions of nonhuman animals. Many animal (and human) actions are directed to objects in the world, including other animals. The notion of directedness can be analysed without attributing intentions or reasons to the agent. An action is directed to object X if and only if: (1) the agent singles out X, either by orientation or by selective performance of the action in the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Cognition, natural selection and the intentional stance.Daisie Radner & Michael Radner - 1995 - International Studies in the Philosophy of Science 9 (2):109-19.
    Abstract Daniel Dennett advocates the use of the intentional stance in adaptationist biology and in cognitive ethology. He sees intentional system theory as closely related to decision theory and game theory. In biological decision and game theory models, nature ?chooses? the strategy by which the animal chooses a course of action. The design of the animal imposes constraints on the model. For Dennett, by contrast, the description of nature's rationale imposes constraints on the design of the animal. Dennett's oversimplified conception (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Hunting memes.H. C. Plotkin - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):768-769.
  • Towards welfare biology: Evolutionary economics of animal consciousness and suffering. [REVIEW]Yew-Kwang Ng - 1995 - Biology and Philosophy 10 (3):255-285.
    Welfare biology is the study of living things and their environment with respect to their welfare. Despite difficulties of ascertaining and measuring welfare and relevancy to normative issues, welfare biology is a positive science. Evolutionary economics and population dynamics are used to help answer basic questions in welfare biology : Which species are affective sentients capable of welfare? Do they enjoy positive or negative welfare? Can their welfare be dramatically increased? Under plausible axioms, all conscious species are plastic and all (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   47 citations  
  • Animal minds and the possession of concepts.Albert Newen & Andreas Bartels - 2007 - Philosophical Psychology 20 (3):283 – 308.
    In the recent literature on concepts, two extreme positions concerning animal minds are predominant: the one that animals possess neither concepts nor beliefs, and the one that some animals possess concepts as well as beliefs. A characteristic feature of this controversy is the lack of consensus on the criteria for possessing a concept or having a belief. Addressing this deficit, we propose a new theory of concepts which takes recent case studies of complex animal behavior into account. The main aim (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   50 citations  
  • Anthropomorphism in the Context of Scientific Discovery: Implications for Comparative Cognition.Farshad Nemati - 2023 - Foundations of Science 28 (3):927-945.
    Mentalist view began to lose its standing among psychologists mainly during the first half of the twentieth century. As a result, the enthusiasm to build an objective science began to grow among behaviourists and ethologists. The rise of cognitive sciences around the 1960s, however, revived the debates over the importance of cognitive intervening variables in explaining behaviours that could not be explained by clinging solely to a pure behavioural approach. Nevertheless, even though cognitive functions in nonhuman animals have been identified (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Apes have mimetic culture.Robert W. Mitchell & H. Lyn Miles - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):768-768.
  • Why Was Thomas A. Sebeok Not a Cognitive Ethologist? From “Animal Mind” to “Semiotic Self”.Timo Maran - 2010 - Biosemiotics 3 (3):315-329.
    In the current debates about zoosemiotics its relations with the neighbouring disciplines are a relevant topic. The present article aims to analyse the complex relations between zoosemiotics and cognitive ethology with special attention to their establishers: Thomas A. Sebeok and Donald R. Griffin. It is argued that zoosemiotics and cognitive ethology have common roots in comparative studies of animal communication in the early 1960s. For supporting this claim Sebeok’s works are analysed, the classical and philosophical periods of his zoosemiotic views (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Correct data base: Wrong model?Alexander Marshack - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):767-768.
  • Maternal encouragement in nonhuman primates and the question of animal teaching.Dario Maestripieri - 1995 - Human Nature 6 (4):361-378.
    Most putative cases of teaching in nonhuman animals involve parent-offspring interactions. The interpretation of these cases, particularly with regard to the cognitive processes involved, is controversial. Qualitative and quantitative observations made in nonhuman primates suggest that, in some species, mothers encourage their infants’ independent locomotion and that encouragement can be considered a form of instruction. In macaques, experience in raising previous offspring accounts in part for variability between mothers in propensity to encourage infant motor skills. Parsimony suggests that the cognitive (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Lessons from evolution for artificial intelligence?Rudi Lutz - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):766-766.
  • Language equals mimesis plus speech.Aarre Laakso - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):765-766.
  • The gradual evolution of enhanced control by plans: A view from below.Leonard D. Katz - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):764-765.
  • In search of human uniqueness.Gary J. Purpura - 2006 - Philosophical Psychology 19 (4):443 – 461.
    Typically in the philosophical literature, kinds of minds are differentiated by the range of cognitive tasks animals accomplish as opposed to the means by which they accomplish the tasks. Drawing on progress in cognitive ethology (the study of animal cognition), I argue that such an approach provides bad directions for uncovering the mark of the human mind. If the goal is to determine what makes the human mind unique, philosophers should focus on the means by which animals interact with objects (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The evolved mind.Harry J. Jerison - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):763-764.
  • Teaching ethics in science and engineering: Animals in research.Dale Jamieson - 1995 - Science and Engineering Ethics 1 (2):185-186.
  • Implicit assumptions about implicit learning.Keith J. Holyoak & Merideth Gattis - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):406-407.
  • Knowing Waste: Towards an Inhuman Epistemology.Myra J. Hird - 2012 - Social Epistemology 26 (3-4):453-469.
    Ten years after the publication of the special issue of Social Epistemology on feminist epistemology, this paper explores recent feminist interest in the inhuman. Feminist science studies, cultural studies, philosophy and environmental studies all build on the important work feminist epistemology has done to bring to the fore questions of feminist empiricism, situated knowledges and knowing as an intersubjective activity. Current research in feminist theory is expanding this epistemological horizon to consider the possibility of an inhuman epistemology. This paper explores (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • From mimetic to mythic culture: Stimulus equivalence effects and prelinguistic cognition.P. J. Hampson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):763-763.
  • Mythos and logos.John Halverson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):762-762.
  • The evolution of pretence: From intentional availability to intentional non-existence.Juan-Carlos Gómez - 2008 - Mind and Language 23 (5):586-606.
    Abstract: I address the issue of how pretence emerged in evolution by reviewing the (mostly negative) evidence about pretend behaviour in non-human primates, and proposing a model of the type of information processing abilities that humans had to evolve in order to be able to pretend. Non-human primates do not typically pretend: there are just a few examples of potential pretend actions mostly produced by apes. The best, but still rare, examples are produced by so-called 'enculturated' apes (reared by humans) (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Working memory and its extensions.K. J. Gilhooly - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):761-762.
  • Cultural transitions occur when mind parasites learn new tricks.Liane M. Gabora - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):760-761.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Evolution needs a modern theory of the mind.James H. Fetzer - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):759-760.
  • From mimesis to synthesis.Jerome A. Feldman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):759-759.
  • Implicit practical learning.Elizabeth Ennen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):404-405.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The modern mind: Its missing parts?R. I. M. Dunbar - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):758-759.
  • Animal beliefs and their contents.Frank Dreckmann - 1999 - Erkenntnis 51 (1):597-615.
    This paper investigates whether, or not, the behavior of animals without speech can manifest beliefs and desires. Criteria for the attribution of such beliefs and desires are worked out with reference to Jonathan Bennett's theory of cognitive teleology: A particular ability for learning justifies attributing such beliefs and desires. The conceptual analysis is illustrated by examinations of cognitive ethology and considers higher-order intentionality. It is argued that the behavioral evidence only supports the attribution of first order beliefs and that languageless (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Précis of Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition.Merlin Donald - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):737-748.
    This bold and brilliant book asks the ultimate question of the life sciences: How did the human mind acquire its incomparable power? In seeking the answer, Merlin Donald traces the evolution of human culture and cognition from primitive apes to the era of artificial intelligence, and presents an original theory of how the human mind evolved from its presymbolic form. In the emergence of modern human culture, Donald proposes, there were three radical transitions. During the first, our bipedal but still (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   129 citations  
  • On the evolution of representational capacities.Merlin Donald - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):775-791.
  • What about pictures?J. B. Deregowski - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):757-758.
  • Ethological foxes and cognitive hedgehogs.Jeffrey Cynx & Stephen J. Clark - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):756-757.
  • Human evolution: Emergence of the group-self.Vilmos Csányi - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):755-756.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The place of cognition in human evolution.Alan Costall - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):755-755.
  • Awareness and abstraction are graded dimensions.Axel Cleeremans - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):402-403.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Symbolic invention: The missing (computational) link?Andy Clark - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):753-754.