Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. A new abstract code or the new possibility of multiple codes?Annette Karmiloff Smith - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):149-150.
  • What kind of indirect process is visual perception?Aaron Sloman - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):401-404.
  • How to turn an information processor into an understander.Aaron Sloman & Monica Croucher - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):447-448.
  • Deep and shallow simulations.Aaron Sloman - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):548-548.
  • Contingencies and rules.B. F. Skinner - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):607-613.
  • An operant analysis of problem solving.B. F. Skinner - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):583-591.
    Behavior that solves a problem is distinguished by the fact that it changes another part of the solver's behavior and is strengthened when it does so. Problem solving typically involves the construction of discriminative stimuli. Verbal responses produce especially useful stimuli, because they affect other people. As a culture formulates maxims, laws, grammar, and science, its members behave more effectively without direct or prolonged contact with the contingencies thus formulated. The culture solves problems for its members, and does so by (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   47 citations  
  • Towards a computational theory of mood.Laura Sizer - 2000 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 51 (4):743-770.
    Moods have global and profound effects on our thoughts, motivations and behavior. To understand human behavior and cognition fully, we must understand moods. In this paper I critically examine and reject the methodology of conventional ?cognitive theories? of affect. I lay the foundations of a new theory of moods that identifies them with processes of our cognitive functional architecture. Moods differ fundamentally from some of our other affective states and hence require distinct explanatory tools. The computational theory of mood I (...)
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  • The hypothalamus and the impartial perspective.Peter Singer - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):84-85.
  • Knowing children's minds.Michael Siegal - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):79-80.
  • Special access lies down with theory-theory.Sydney Shoemaker - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):78-79.
  • Historicism, behaviorism, and the conceptual status of memory representations in animals.Charles P. Shimp - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):389-390.
  • Metaphor versus reality in the understanding of imagery: the path from function to structure.Peter W. Sheehan - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):567-568.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Expectancy: The endogenous source of anticipatory activities, including “pseudoconditioned” responses.Patrick J. Sheafor - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):387-389.
  • The image-like and the language-like.Benny Shanon - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):566-567.
  • Representations - senses and reasons.Benny Shanon - 1991 - Philosophical Psychology 4 (3):355-74.
    Abstract A survey of different senses of the term ?representation? is presented. The presentation is guided by the appraisal that this key term is employed in the cognitive literature in different senses and that the distinction between these is not always explicitly stated or appreciated. Furthermore, the different senses seem to be associated with different rationales for the postulation of representation. Given that there may be a lack of convergence between the various senses of the construct in question and the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Meno—a Cognitive Psychological View.Benny Shanon - 1984 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 35 (2):129-147.
  • Junk Representations.Lawrence A. Shapiro - 1997 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 48 (3):345-361.
    Many philosophers and psychologists who approach the issue of representation from a computational or measurement theoretical perspective end up having to deny the possibility of junk representations—representations present in an organism's head but that enter into no psychological processes or produce no behaviour. However, I argue, a more functional perspective makes the possibility of junk representations intuitively quite plausible—so much so that we may wish to question those views of representation that preclude the possibility of junk representations. I explore some (...)
    Direct download (13 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Consciousness beyond the comparator.Victor A. Shames & Timothy L. Hubbard - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):697-697.
    Gray's comparator model fails to provide an adequate explanation of consciousness for two reasons. First, it is based on a narrow definition of consciousness that excludes basic phenomenology and active functions of consciousness. Second, match/mismatch decisions can be made without producing an experience of consciousness. The model thus violates the sufficiency criterion.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Abstract machine theory and direct perception.Robert Shaw & James Todd - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):400-401.
  • The analytic/holistic dichotomy: An epiphenomenon.Justine Sergent - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):521.
  • Levels of research.Colleen Seifert & Donald A. Norman - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):490-492.
  • Metatheory of animal behavior.Erwin M. Segal - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):386-387.
  • The Chinese room revisited.J. R. Searle - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (2):345-348.
  • Intrinsic intentionality.John Searle - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):450-457.
  • Understanding Searle.Roger C. Schank - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):446-447.
  • The ecological approach to social perception: A conceptual critique.Bernd H. Schmitt - 1987 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 17 (3):265–278.
  • Hallucination, rationalization, and response set.Steven Schwartz - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):532-533.
  • Communication and consciousness: A neural network conjecture.N. A. Schmajuk & E. Axelrad - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):695-696.
    The communicative aspects of the contents of consciousness are analyzed in the framework of a neural network model of animal communication. We discuss some issues raised by Gray, such as the control of the contents of consciousness, the adaptive value of consciousness, conscious and unconscious behaviors, and the nature of a model's consciousness.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Al, imagery, and theories.Roger C. Schank - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):566-566.
  • New wine in old glasses?Joseph M. Scandura - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):602-603.
  • Intentionality and communication theory.K. M. Sayre - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):155-165.
  • Intentionality and information processing: An alternative model for cognitive science.Kenneth M. Sayre - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):121-38.
    This article responds to two unresolved and crucial problems of cognitive science: (1) What is actually accomplished by functions of the nervous system that we ordinarily describe in the intentional idiom? and (2) What makes the information processing involved in these functions semantic? It is argued that, contrary to the assumptions of many cognitive theorists, the computational approach does not provide coherent answers to these problems, and that a more promising start would be to fall back on mathematical communication theory (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   75 citations  
  • Unjustified presuppositions of competence.Leah Savion - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):364-365.
  • Searle's demon and the brain simulator.Steven F. Savitt - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (2):342-343.
  • Scotch'd the snake, not killed it.Peter T. Saunders & Mae-Wan Ho - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):83-84.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Disenshrining the Cartesian self.Barbara A. C. Saunders - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):77-78.
  • On demystifying the mental for psychology.Edward Sankowski - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):565-566.
  • Circulation as consciousness.Curt A. Sandman - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):302-304.
  • Understanding and integration.Jerry Samet - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (2):341-342.
  • Grammars-as-programs versus grammars- as-data.Jerry Samet - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):414-414.
  • Pop sociobiology and meta-ethics.Merrilee H. Salmon - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):83-83.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On leaving your children wrapped in thought.James Russell - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):76-77.
  • Verbal hallucinations and information processing.Bjørn Rishovd Rund - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):531-532.
  • There is more to psychological meaningfulness than computation and representation.Sverker Runeson - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):399-400.
  • The logic of representation.William W. Rozeboom - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):385-386.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On theory and metatheory, and normal and revolutionary science.Joseph R. Royce - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):599-599.
  • Weak versus strong claims about the algorithmic level.Paul S. Rosenbloom - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):490-490.
  • Is there really “juggling,” “artifice,” and “trickery” in Genes, Mind, and Culture?Alexander Rosenberg - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):80-82.
  • Searle and the special powers of the brain.Richard Rorty - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):445-446.
  • Meaning, Context, and Control:Convergent trends and controversial issues in current Social‐scientific research on Human cognition and communication.Ragnar Rommetveit - 1987 - Inquiry: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy 30 (1 & 2):77 – 99.
    A survey of a wide range of social?scientific disciplines reveals a definite convergence of theoretical interest in human cognition and communication as situated, concerned, and embedded in social commitment. Recent contributions within situation semantics and cognitive science explicitly reject some of the constraints inherent in their shared philosophical heritage and prepare novel ground for dialogues between fields as far apart as formal semantics and ?dialogical? text theory. Issues such as purely cognitive versus motivational aspects of human situatedness, and the relationship (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations