Switch to: References

Citations of:

Structuralism

New York,: Basic Books (1970)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The Process of Reflective Teaching.Peter Silcock - 1994 - British Journal of Educational Studies 42 (3):273 - 285.
    The process of reflection is analysed into three components - an ego-driven purpose, a restructuring capability, and a transforming perspective. Different types of reflection are argued to be instances of cognitive restructuring determined by purpose and by context. Procedures for resolving contradictions in the literature concerning ways in which 'reflective teaching' can be fostered are also suggested. It is argued that adopting any single model of 'reflective practice' can be unnecessarily restrictive given the ubiquity of the reflective process. Finally, the (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Structure and history in the semiotics of myth.William Hendricks - 1982 - Semiotica 39 (1/2):131-165.
    The structure of narrative discourse is the focus of much current research, but the classicist Walter Burkert argues for a revitalization of the historical approach to myth. He pushes the origins of myth beyond ritual to action patterns man shares with animals. His approach is evaluated in the context of Propp's historical analysis, which complements his 'morphological' approach to Russian folktales; and to Levi-Strauss's synchronic analysis of myth, which sees an ahistoric connection between the myths of North and South America.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The anatomy of knowledge: Althusser's epistemology and its consequences.D. Atkinson - 1984 - Philosophical Papers 13 (2):1-18.
  • Waking Up and Growing Up: Two Forms of Human Development.Blaine Snow - manuscript
    This paper contrasts two relatively independent forms of human development: waking up, the process and practices of psychospiritual awakening , and growing up, the process of moving from lesser narcissistic and ethnocentric self-identities towards mature postconventional self-identities with greater degrees of inclusion, perspective-taking, caring, and compassion. Each is a unique type of growth, contemplative and transformative, with different ways of engaging and differing goals and results. The former is about transcending or deconstructing the ego and the latter about building, strengthening, (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Structuralism in Phylogenetic Systematics.Richard H. Zander - 2010 - Biological Theory 5 (4):383-394.
    Systematics based solely on structuralist principles is non-science because it is derived from first principles that are inconsistent in dealing with both synchronic and diachronic aspects of evolution, and its evolutionary models involve hidden causes, and unnameable and unobservable entities. Structuralist phylogenetics emulates axiomatic mathematics through emphasis on deduction, and “hypotheses” and “mapped trait changes” that are actually lemmas and theorems. Sister-group-only evolutionary trees have no caulistic element of scientific realism. This results in a degenerate systematics based on patterns of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the necessity of an archetypal concept in morphology: With special reference to the concepts of “structure” and “homology”. [REVIEW]Bruce A. Young - 1993 - Biology and Philosophy 8 (2):225-248.
    Morphological elements, or structures, are sorted into four categories depending on their level of anatomical isolation and the presence or absence of intrinsically identifying characteristics. These four categories are used to highlight the difficulties with the concept of structure and our ability to identify or define structures. The analysis is extended to the concept of homology through a discussion of the methodological and philosophical problems of the current concept of homology. It is argued that homology is fundamentally a similarity based (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Attention to detail?Malcolm P. Young, Ian R. Paterson & David I. Perrett - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):417-418.
  • Where's the psychological reality?C. Philip Winder - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):417-417.
  • Modelling the psychological structure of reasoning.M. A. Winstanley - 2022 - European Journal for Philosophy of Science 12 (2):1-27.
    Mathematics and logic are indispensable in science, yet how they are deployed and why they are so effective, especially in the natural sciences, is poorly understood. In this paper, I focus on the how by analysing Jean Piaget’s application of mathematics to the empirical content of psychological experiment; however, I do not lose sight of the application’s wider implications on the why. In a case study, I set out how Piaget drew on the stock of mathematical structures to model psychological (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A psychological theory of reasoning as logical evidence: a Piagetian perspective.M. A. Winstanley - 2021 - Synthese 199 (3-4):10077-10108.
    Many contemporary logicians acknowledge a plurality of logical theories and accept that theory choice is in part motivated by logical evidence. However, just as there is no agreement on logical theories, there is also no consensus on what constitutes logical evidence. In this paper, I outline Jean Piaget’s psychological theory of reasoning and show how he used it to diagnose and solve one of the paradoxes of material implication. I assess Piaget’s use of psychology as a source of evidence for (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Is extension to perception of real-world objects and scenes possible?J. Wagemans, K. Verfaillie, P. De Graef & K. Lamberts - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):415-417.
  • Science Teaching: What Does It Mean?Michael Tseitlin & Igal Galili - 2006 - Science & Education 15 (5):393-417.
  • Harré's social being: The connection with structuralism.Sally Swartz & Leslie Swartz - 1982 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 12 (2):201–212.
  • Is the tag necessary?Ron Sun & Emmanuel Schalit - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):415-415.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Understanding topological relationships through comparisons of similar knots.Carol Strohecker - 1996 - AI and Society 10 (1):58-69.
    This paper examines an example of learning with artifacts using the commonplace materials of string and knots. Emphases include research into learning processes as well as construction of objects to assist learning. The inquiry concerns the development of mathematical thinking, topology in particular. The research methodology combines participant observation and clinical interview within a constructionist framework. The study was set in a self-styled, self-constructed environment that consisted of knots and a social substrate encouraging lively exchanges of ideas about them. Comparisons (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The value of modeling visual attention.Gary W. Strong & Bruce A. Whitehead - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):419-433.
  • A solution to the tag-assignment problem for neural networks.Gary W. Strong & Bruce A. Whitehead - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):381-397.
    Purely parallel neural networks can model object recognition in brief displays – the same conditions under which illusory conjunctions have been demonstrated empirically. Correcting errors of illusory conjunction is the “tag-assignment” problem for a purely parallel processor: the problem of assigning a spatial tag to nonspatial features, feature combinations, and objects. This problem must be solved to model human object recognition over a longer time scale. Our model simulates both the parallel processes that may underlie illusory conjunctions and the serial (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   175 citations  
  • An attentional hierarchy.Peter A. Sandon - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):414-415.
  • Theorizing Political Psychology: Doing Integrative Social Science Under the Condition of Postmodernity.Shawn W. Rosenberg - 2003 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 33 (4):427-459.
    The field of political psychology, like the social sciences more generally, is being challenged. New theoretical direction is being demanded from within and a greater epistemological sophistication and ethical relevance is being demanded from without. In response, an outline for a reconstructed political psychology is offered here. To begin, a theoretical framework for a truly integrative political psychology is sketched. In the attempt to transcend the reductionist quality of cross-disciplinary or multidisciplinary inquiry, the theoretical approach offered here emphasizes the dually (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Constructivism and logical reasoning.Barry Richards - 1985 - Synthese 65 (1):33 - 64.
  • Constructional morphology: The analysis of constraints in evolution dedicated to A. seilacher in honour of his 60. birthday.Wolf-Ernst Reify, Roger D. K. Thomas & Martin S. Fischer - 1985 - Acta Biotheoretica 34 (2-4):233-248.
    Evolutionary change is opportunistic, but its course is strongly constrained in several fundamental ways. These constraints (historical/phylogenetic, functional/adaptive, constructional/morphogenetic) and their dynamic relationships are discussed here and shown to constitute the conceptual framework of Constructional Morphology. Notwithstanding recent published opinions which claim that the discovery of constraints renders Neodarwinian selection theory obsolete, we regard the insights of Constructional Morphology as being entirely consistent with this theory. As is shown here in the case of the Hyracoidea, formal analysis of the constraints (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Damn the (behavioral) data, full steam ahead.William Prinzmetal & Richard Ivry - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):413-414.
  • Hide And Seek: Values In Early Childhood Education And Care.Sacha Powell - 2010 - British Journal of Educational Studies 58 (2):213-229.
    Early childhood education and care settings in England and the people who work in them constitute an important sphere of influence, shaping young children's characters and values. But the values and dispositions expected of the early years workforce are missing from statutory policy documentation despite its clear requirement that practitioners will espouse and promote particular 'universal' values in their work. The expectations for young children's achievement by the age of five also reveal a complex approach combining a structuralist and postmodern (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neural networks and computational theory: Solving the right problem.David C. Plaut - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):411-413.
  • Towards a metaphorical biology.R. C. Paton - 1992 - Biology and Philosophy 7 (3):279-294.
    The metaphorical nature of biological language is examined and the use of metaphors for providing the linguistic context in which similarities and differences are made is described. Certain pervasive metaphors which are characterised by systemic properties are noted, and in order to provide some focus to the study, systemic metaphors associated with machine, text and organism are discussed. Other systemic metaphors such as society and circuit are also reported. Some details concerning interrelations between automaton and organism are presented in the (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Simultaneous processing of features may not be possible.D. M. Parker - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):411-411.
  • Foucault's anti-humanism.Roger Paden - 1987 - Human Studies 10 (1):123 - 141.
  • A structuralist view of explanation: a critique of Brainerd.David R. Olson - 1978 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1 (2):197-199.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Co-generic logic as a theoretical framework for the analysis of communication in living systems.Yair Neuman - 2003 - Semiotica 2003 (144):49-65.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Why the Logical Hexagon?Alessio Moretti - 2012 - Logica Universalis 6 (1-2):69-107.
    The logical hexagon (or hexagon of opposition) is a strange, yet beautiful, highly symmetrical mathematical figure, mysteriously intertwining fundamental logical and geometrical features. It was discovered more or less at the same time (i.e. around 1950), independently, by a few scholars. It is the successor of an equally strange (but mathematically less impressive) structure, the “logical square” (or “square of opposition”), of which it is a much more general and powerful “relative”. The discovery of the former did not raise interest, (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Fundamental design limitations in tag assignment.Hermann J. Müller, Glyn W. Humphreys, Philip T. Quinlan & Nick Donnelly - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):410-411.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The direct perception of universals: A theory of knowledge acquisition.Viki McCabe - 1982 - Synthese 52 (3):495 - 513.
    A theory is presented which proposes that knowledge acquisition involves direct perception of schematic information in the form of structural and transformational invariances. Individual components with salient verbal descriptions are considered conscious place-holders for non-conscious invariant schemes. It is speculated that theories positing mental construction have three related causes: The first is a lack of consciousness of the schema processing capacities of the right hemisphere; the second is the paucity of adequate words to express schematic relationships; and the last involves (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The concept of domain in developmental analyses of hierarchical complexity.Michael F. Mascolo - 2008 - World Futures 64 (5-7):330 – 347.
    Individuals do not operate “at a stage of development.” They operate at a range of different levels of hierarchical complexity depending on skill area, task, context, degree of support, and other variables. It is thus necessary to postulate the concept of domain to refer to the particular conceptual, behavioral, or affective area within which activity operates. The concept raises questions and implications for theory building and application. Such issues are elaborated by discussing a variety of domains and social contexts. A (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • From Alan Turing to modern AI: practical solutions and an implicit epistemic stance.George F. Luger & Chayan Chakrabarti - 2017 - AI and Society 32 (3):321-338.
    It has been just over 100 years since the birth of Alan Turing and more than 65 years since he published in Mind his seminal paper, Computing Machinery and Intelligence. In the Mind paper, Turing asked a number of questions, including whether computers could ever be said to have the power of “thinking”. Turing also set up a number of criteria—including his imitation game—under which a human could judge whether a computer could be said to be “intelligent”. Turing’s paper, as (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Re‐conceptualizing Abstract Conceptualization in Social Theory: The Case of the “Structure” Concept.Omar Lizardo - 2013 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 43 (2):155-180.
    I this paper, I draw on recent research on the radically embodied and perceptual bases of conceptualization in linguistics and cognitive science to develop a new way of reading and evaluating abstract concepts in social theory. I call this approach Sociological Idea Analysis. I argue that, in contrast to the traditional view of abstract concepts, which conceives them as amodal “presuppositions” removed from experience, abstract concepts are irreducibly grounded in experience and partake of non-negotiable perceptual-symbolic features from which a non-propositional (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Beyond the antinomies of structure: Levi-Strauss, Giddens, Bourdieu, and Sewell. [REVIEW]Omar Lizardo - 2010 - Theory and Society 39 (6):651-688.
  • A self-organizing perceptual system.James R. Levenick - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):409-410.
  • A nonspatial solution to a spatial problem.Ronald M. Lesperance & Stephen Kaplan - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):408-409.
  • Epistemological Problems and the Personality and Social System in Social Psychology.Hans Lenk - 1975 - Theory and Decision 6 (1/4):333.
  • State transitions in constraint satisfaction networks.John K. Kruschke - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):407-408.
  • Features and locations: Dichotomy or continuum?Lester E. Krueger & Leann M. Stadtlander - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):406-407.
  • Michel Foucault's archaeology of knowledge and economic discourse.Serhat Kologlugil - 2010 - Erasmus Journal for Philosophy and Economics 3 (2):1.
    The literature in economic methodology has witnessed an increase in the number of studies which, drawing upon the postmodern turn in social sciences, pay serious attention to the non-epistemological-discursive elements of economic theorizing. This recent work on the "economic discourse" has thus added a new dimension to economic methodology by analyzing various discursive aspects of the construction of scientific meanings in economics. Taking a similar stance, this paper explores Michel Foucault's archaeological analysis of scientific discourses. It aims to show that (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Piaget's epistemic subject and science education: Epistemological vs. psychological issues.Richard F. Kitchener - 1993 - Science & Education 2 (2):137-148.
  • Review. [REVIEW]Richard F. Kitchener - 1991 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 42 (2):285-290.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Piaget's social psychology.Richard F. Kitchener - 1981 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 11 (3):253–277.
    Piaget's social psychology is not widely discussed among psychologists, partly because much of it is still contained in untranslated French works. In this article I summarize the main lines of Piaget's social psychology and briefly indicate its relation to current theories in social psychology. Rejecting both Durkheim's sociological holism and Tarde's individualism, Piaget advances a sociological relativism in which all social facts are reducible to social relations and these, in turn, are reducible to rules, values and signs. Piaget's theory of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Genetic epistemology and the prospects for a cognitive sociology of science: A critical synthesis.Richard Kitchener - 1989 - Social Epistemology 3 (2):153 – 169.
  • Transformational linguistics and structural anthropology.Roger M. Keesing - 1975 - Philosophy and Social Criticism 2 (3):243-266.
  • What is a human? Toward psychological benchmarks in the field of humanrobot interaction.Peter H. Kahn, Hiroshi Ishiguro, Batya Friedman, Takayuki Kanda, Nathan G. Freier, Rachel L. Severson & Jessica Miller - 2007 - Interaction Studies 8 (3):363-390.
  • In moral relationship with nature: Development and interaction.Peter H. Kahn - 2022 - Journal of Moral Education 51 (1):73-91.
    ABSTRACT One of the overarching problems of the world today is that too many people see themselves as dominating other groups of people, and dominating nature. That is a root problem. And thus part of a core solution builds from Kohlberg’s commitment to a universal moral orientation, though extended to include not only all people but the more-than-human world: animals, trees, plants, species, ecosystems, and the land itself. In this article, I make a case for this form of ethical extensionism, (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Social representations in and of the public sphere: Towards a theoretical articulation.Sandra Jovchelovitch - 1995 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 25 (1):81–102.