Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Précis of Doing without Concepts.Edouard Machery - 2010 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33 (2-3):195-206.
    Although cognitive scientists have learned a lot about concepts, their findings have yet to be organized in a coherent theoretical framework. In addition, after twenty years of controversy, there is little sign that philosophers and psychologists are converging toward an agreement about the very nature of concepts.Doing without Concepts(Machery 2009) attempts to remedy this state of affairs. In this article, I review the main points and arguments developed at greater length inDoing without Concepts.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • The Information‐Processing Perspective on Categorization.Manolo Martínez - 2024 - Cognitive Science 48 (2):e13411.
    Categorization behavior can be fruitfully analyzed in terms of the trade‐off between as high as possible faithfulness in the transmission of information about samples of the classes to be categorized, and as low as possible transmission costs for that same information. The kinds of categorization behaviors we associate with conceptual atoms, prototypes, and exemplars emerge naturally as a result of this trade‐off, in the presence of certain natural constraints on the probabilistic distribution of samples, and the ways in which we (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • When more is less: Feedback effects in perceptual category learning.J. Vincent Filoteo W. Todd Maddox, Bradley C. Love, Brian D. Glass - 2008 - Cognition 108 (2):578.
  • On the creation of classification systems of memory.Daniel B. Willingham - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):426-427.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • One and Done? Optimal Decisions From Very Few Samples.Edward Vul, Noah Goodman, Thomas L. Griffiths & Joshua B. Tenenbaum - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (4):599-637.
    In many learning or inference tasks human behavior approximates that of a Bayesian ideal observer, suggesting that, at some level, cognition can be described as Bayesian inference. However, a number of findings have highlighted an intriguing mismatch between human behavior and standard assumptions about optimality: People often appear to make decisions based on just one or a few samples from the appropriate posterior probability distribution, rather than using the full distribution. Although sampling-based approximations are a common way to implement Bayesian (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   53 citations  
  • Do Additional Features Help or Hurt Category Learning? The Curse of Dimensionality in Human Learners.Wai Keen Vong, Andrew T. Hendrickson, Danielle J. Navarro & Amy Perfors - 2019 - Cognitive Science 43 (3):e12724.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Do Additional Features Help or Hurt Category Learning? The Curse of Dimensionality in Human Learners.Wai Keen Vong, Andrew T. Hendrickson, Danielle J. Navarro & Andrew Perfors - 2019 - Cognitive Science 43 (3).
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Developing Representations of Compound Stimuli.Ingmar Visser & Maartje E. J. Raijmakers - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The GIST of concepts.Ronaldo Vigo - 2013 - Cognition 129 (1):138-162.
  • Similarity and Rules United: Similarity‐ and Rule‐Based Processing in a Single Neural Network.Tom Verguts & Wim Fias - 2009 - Cognitive Science 33 (2):243-259.
    A central controversy in cognitive science concerns the roles of rules versus similarity. To gain some leverage on this problem, we propose that rule‐ versus similarity‐based processes can be characterized as extremes in a multidimensional space that is composed of at least two dimensions: the number of features (Pothos, 2005) and the physical presence of features. The transition of similarity‐ to rule‐based processing is conceptualized as a transition in this space. To illustrate this, we show how a neural network model (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A note on DeCaro, Thomas, and Beilock (2008): Further data demonstrate complexities in the assessment of information–integration category learning.Ian J. Tharp & Alan D. Pickering - 2009 - Cognition 111 (3):410-414.
  • Are infants human?H. S. Terrace - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):425-426.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is awareness necessary for operant conditioning?Frode Svartdal - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):424-425.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Interaction of the Explicit and the Implicit in Skill Learning: A Dual-Process Approach.Ron Sun - 2005 - Psychological Review 112 (1):159-192.
    This article explicates the interaction between implicit and explicit processes in skill learning, in contrast to the tendency of researchers to study each type in isolation. It highlights various effects of the interaction on learning (including synergy effects). The authors argue for an integrated model of skill learning that takes into account both implicit and explicit processes. Moreover, they argue for a bottom-up approach (first learning implicit knowledge and then explicit knowledge) in the integrated model. A variety of qualitative data (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   55 citations  
  • Computational Models of Consciousness: An Evaluation.Ron Sun - 1999 - Journal of Intelligent Systems 9 (5-6):507-568.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Accounting for the computational basis of consciousness: A connectionist approach.Ron Sun - 1999 - Consciousness and Cognition 8 (4):529-565.
    This paper argues for an explanation of the mechanistic (computational) basis of consciousness that is based on the distinction between localist (symbolic) representation and distributed representation, the ideas of which have been put forth in the connectionist literature. A model is developed to substantiate and test this approach. The paper also explores the issue of the functional roles of consciousness, in relation to the proposed mechanistic explanation of consciousness. The model, embodying the representational difference, is able to account for the (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Whither learning, whither memory?Michael A. Stadler & Peter A. Frensch - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):423-424.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Dissociable learning and memory systems of the brain.Larry R. Squire, Stephan Hamann & Barbara Knowlton - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):422-423.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Alternative strategies of categorization.Edward E. Smith, Andrea L. Patalano & John Jonides - 1998 - Cognition 65 (2-3):167-196.
  • How should implicit learning be characterized?David R. Shanks & Mark F. St John - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):427-447.
  • Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems.David R. Shanks & Mark F. St John - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):367-447.
    A number of ways of taxonomizing human learning have been proposed. We examine the evidence for one such proposal, namely, that there exist independent explicit and implicit learning systems. This combines two further distinctions, (1) between learning that takes place with versus without concurrent awareness, and (2) between learning that involves the encoding of instances (or fragments) versus the induction of abstract rules or hypotheses. Implicit learning is assumed to involve unconscious rule learning. We examine the evidence for implicit learning (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   192 citations  
  • Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems.David R. Shanks & Mark F. St John - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):367-395.
    A number of ways of taxonomizing human learning have been proposed. We examine the evidence for one such proposal, namely, that there exist independent explicit and implicit learning systems. This combines two further distinctions, between learning that takes place with versus without concurrent awareness, and between learning that involves the encoding of instances versus the induction of abstract rules or hypotheses. Implicit learning is assumed to involve unconscious rule learning. We examine the evidence for implicit learning derived from subliminal learning, (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   186 citations  
  • A probabilistic model of cross-categorization.Patrick Shafto, Charles Kemp, Vikash Mansinghka & Joshua B. Tenenbaum - 2011 - Cognition 120 (1):1-25.
  • Testing three coping strategies for time pressure in categorizations and similarity judgments.Florian I. Seitz, Bettina von Helversen, Rebecca Albrecht, Jörg Rieskamp & Jana B. Jarecki - 2023 - Cognition 233 (C):105358.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Criteria for implicit learning: Deemphasize conscious access, emphasize amnesia.Carol Augart Seger - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):421-422.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The development of features in object concepts.Philippe G. Schyns, Robert L. Goldstone & Jean-Pierre Thibaut - 1998 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 (1):1-17.
    According to one productive and influential approach to cognition, categorization, object recognition, and higher level cognitive processes operate on a set of fixed features, which are the output of lower level perceptual processes. In many situations, however, it is the higher level cognitive process being executed that influences the lower level features that are created. Rather than viewing the repertoire of features as being fixed by low-level processes, we present a theory in which people create features to subserve the representation (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   98 citations  
  • Testing adaptive toolbox models: A Bayesian hierarchical approach.Benjamin Scheibehenne, Jörg Rieskamp & Eric-Jan Wagenmakers - 2013 - Psychological Review 120 (1):39-64.
  • Eye movements reveal memory processes during similarity- and rule-based decision making.Agnes Scholz, Bettina von Helversen & Jörg Rieskamp - 2015 - Cognition 136 (C):228-246.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Learning strategies and situated knowledge.Antonio Rizzo & Oronzo Parlangeli - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):420-421.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • New evidence for unconscious sequence learning.Jonathan Reed & Peder Johnson - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):419-420.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What manner of mind is this?Arthur S. Reber & Bill Winter - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):418-419.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Learning without awareness: What counts as an appropriate test of learning and of awareness.Sam S. Rakover - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):417-418.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A Cognitive Modeling Approach to Strategy Formation in Dynamic Decision Making.Prezenski Sabine, Brechmann André, Wolff Susann & Russwinkel Nele - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • One or two dimensions in spontaneous classification: A simplicity approach.Emmanuel M. Pothos & James Close - 2008 - Cognition 107 (2):581-602.
  • On the representational/computational properties of multiple memory systems.Russell A. Poldrack & Neal J. Cohen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):416-417.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What about unconscious processing during the test?Pierre Perruchet & Jorge Gallego - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):415-416.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The role of background knowledge in speeded perceptual categorization.Thomas J. Palmeri & Celina Blalock - 2000 - Cognition 77 (2):B45-B57.
  • Dissociating multiple memory systems: Don't forsake the brain.Mark G. Packard - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):414-415.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The intuitive mind.Geir Overskeid - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):414-414.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • An exemplar-based random walk model of speeded classification.Robert M. Nosofsky & Thomas J. Palmeri - 1997 - Psychological Review 104 (2):266-300.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   75 citations  
  • Faulty rationale for the two factors that dissociate learning systems.Hiroshi Nagata - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):412-413.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Do Americans Have a Preference for Rule‐Based Classification?Gregory L. Murphy, David A. Bosch & ShinWoo Kim - 2017 - Cognitive Science:2026-2052.
    Six experiments investigated variables predicted to influence subjects’ tendency to classify items by a single property instead of overall similarity, following the paradigm of Norenzayan et al., who found that European Americans tended to give more “logical” rule-based responses. However, in five experiments with Mechanical Turk subjects and undergraduates at an American university, we found a consistent preference for similarity-based responding. A sixth experiment with Korean undergraduates revealed an effect of instructions, also reported by Norenzayan et al., in which classification (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Phonological Concept Learning.Elliott Moreton, Joe Pater & Katya Pertsova - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (1):4-69.
    Linguistic and non-linguistic pattern learning have been studied separately, but we argue for a comparative approach. Analogous inductive problems arise in phonological and visual pattern learning. Evidence from three experiments shows that human learners can solve them in analogous ways, and that human performance in both cases can be captured by the same models. We test GMECCS, an implementation of the Configural Cue Model in a Maximum Entropy phonotactic-learning framework with a single free parameter, against the alternative hypothesis that learners (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Accounting for Graded Performance within a Discrete Search Framework.Craig S. Miller & John E. Laird - 1996 - Cognitive Science 20 (4):499-537.
    This article presents a process account of some typicality effects and related similarity-dependent accuracy and response time phenomena that arise in the context of supervised concept acquisition. We describe Symbolic Concept Acquisition (SCA), a computational system that acquires and activates category prediction rules. In contrast to gradient representations, SCA performs by probing for prediction rules in a series of discrete steps. For learning new rules, it acquires general rules but then incrementally learns more specific ones. In describing SCA, we emphasize (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • On the Futility of Attempting to Demonstrate Null Awareness.Philip M. Merikle - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):412-412.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • From implicit skills to explicit knowledge: a bottom‐up model of skill learning.Edward Merrillb & Todd Petersonb - 2001 - Cognitive Science 25 (2):203-244.
    This paper presents a skill learning model CLARION. Different from existing models of mostly high-level skill learning that use a top-down approach (that is, turning declarative knowledge into procedural knowledge through practice), we adopt a bottom-up approach toward low-level skill learning, where procedural knowledge develops first and declarative knowledge develops later. Our model is formed by integrating connectionist, reinforcement, and symbolic learning methods to perform on-line reactive learning. It adopts a two-level dual-representation framework (Sun, 1995), with a combination of localist (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   47 citations  
  • Implementational constraints on human learning and memory systems.Chad J. Marsolek - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):411-412.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • When more is less: Feedback effects in perceptual category learning.W. Todd Maddox, Bradley C. Love, Brian D. Glass & J. Vincent Filoteo - 2008 - Cognition 108 (2):578-589.
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Précis of doing without concepts.Edouard Machery - 2010 - Philosophical Studies 149 (3):602-611.
    Although cognitive scientists have learned a lot about concepts, their findings have yet to be organized in a coherent theoretical framework. In addition, after twenty years of controversy, there is little sign that philosophers and psychologists are converging toward an agreement about the very nature of concepts. Doing without Concepts (Machery 2009) attempts to remedy this state of affairs. In this article, I review the main points and arguments developed at greater length in Doing without Concepts.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • The Algorithmic Level Is the Bridge Between Computation and Brain.Bradley C. Love - 2015 - Topics in Cognitive Science 7 (2):230-242.
    Every scientist chooses a preferred level of analysis and this choice shapes the research program, even determining what counts as evidence. This contribution revisits Marr's three levels of analysis and evaluates the prospect of making progress at each individual level. After reviewing limitations of theorizing within a level, two strategies for integration across levels are considered. One is top–down in that it attempts to build a bridge from the computational to algorithmic level. Limitations of this approach include insufficient theoretical constraint (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations