Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Counting to ten milliseconds: Low-anger, but not high-anger, individuals pause following negative evaluations.Michael D. Robinson, Benjamin M. Wilkowski, Brian P. Meier, Sara K. Moeller & Adam K. Fetterman - 2012 - Cognition and Emotion 26 (2):261-281.
    The emotion of anger, when chronic, is especially problematic. Frequent and intense experiences of anger predict quite a few adverse health outcomes and are especially implicated in cardiovascular...
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Response-bound primes diminish affective priming in the naming task.Dirk Wentura & Christian Frings - 2008 - Cognition and Emotion 22 (2):374-384.
  • Ignoring ''brutal'' will make ''numid'' more pleasant but ''uyuvu'' more unpleasant: The role of a priori pleasantness of unfamiliar stimuli in affective priming tasks.Dirk Wentura - 2002 - Cognition and Emotion 16 (2):269-298.
  • Automatic evaluation isn't that crude! Moderation of masked affective priming by type of valence.Dirk Wentura & Juliane Degner - 2010 - Cognition and Emotion 24 (4):609-628.
  • Controlling the “uncontrollable”: Faking effects on the affect misattribution procedure.Sarah Teige-Mocigemba, Barnabas Penzl, Manuel Becker, Laura Henn & Karl Christoph Klauer - 2016 - Cognition and Emotion 30 (8).
  • Masked emotional priming beyond global valence activations.Michaela Rohr, Juliane Degner & Dirk Wentura - 2012 - Cognition and Emotion 26 (2):224-244.
  • Are self-deceivers enhancing positive affect or denying negative affect? Toward an understanding of implicit affective processes.Michael D. Robinson, Sara K. Moeller & Paul W. Goetz - 2009 - Cognition and Emotion 23 (1):152-180.
    Self-deception is an important construct in social, personality, and clinical literatures. Although historical and clinical views of self-deception have regarded it as defensive in nature and operation, modern views of this individual difference variable instead highlight its apparent benefits to subjective mental health. The present four studies reinforce the latter view by showing that self-deception predicts positive priming effects, but not negative priming effects, in reaction time tasks sensitive to individual differences in affective priming. In all studies, individuals higher in (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Living large: Affect amplification in visual perception predicts emotional reactivity to events in daily life.Spencer L. Palder, Scott Ode, Tianwei Liu & Michael D. Robinson - 2013 - Cognition and Emotion 27 (3):453-464.
  • Brightness differences influence the evaluation of affective pictures.Daniël Lakens, Daniel A. Fockenberg, Karin P. H. Lemmens, Jaap Ham & Cees J. H. Midden - 2013 - Cognition and Emotion 27 (7):1225-1246.
  • Implicit emotion regulation under demanding conditions: The moderating role of action versus state orientation.Sander L. Koole & Daniel A. Fockenberg - 2011 - Cognition and Emotion 25 (3):440-452.
  • Affective processing in overwhelmed individuals: Strategic and task considerations.John G. Kerns & Howard Berenbaum - 2010 - Cognition and Emotion 24 (4):638-660.
  • Processing negative valence of word pairs that include a positive word.Oksana Itkes & Nira Mashal - 2016 - Cognition and Emotion 30 (6).
  • The costs and benefits of processing emotional stimuli during rapid serial visual presentation.Niklas Ihssen & Andreas Keil - 2009 - Cognition and Emotion 23 (2):296-326.
  • Early adolescents show sustained susceptibility to cognitive interference by emotional distractors.Sabine Heim, Niklas Ihssen, Marcus Hasselhorn & Andreas Keil - 2013 - Cognition and Emotion 27 (4):696-706.
  • Attentional influences on affective priming: Does categorisation influence spontaneous evaluations of multiply categorisable objects?Bertram Gawronski, William A. Cunningham, Etienne P. LeBel & Roland Deutsch - 2010 - Cognition and Emotion 24 (6):1008-1025.
  • Dysphorics cannot ignore unpleasant information.Christian Frings, Dirk Wentura & Maike Holtz - 2007 - Cognition and Emotion 21 (7):1525-1534.
  • Fast and unintentional evaluation of emotional sounds: evidence from brief segment ratings and the affective Simon task.Tímea Folyi & Dirk Wentura - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 31 (2).
  • On the malleability of automatic attentional biases: Effects of feature-specific attention allocation.Tom Everaert, Adriaan Spruyt & Jan De Houwer - 2013 - Cognition and Emotion 27 (3):385-400.
  • How distinctive is affective processing? On the implications of using cognitive paradigms to study affect and emotion.Andreas B. Eder, Bernhard Hommel & Jan De Houwer - 2007 - Cognition and Emotion 21 (6):1137-1154.
    Influential theories on affect and emotion propose a fundamental differentiation between emotion and cognition, and research paradigms designed to test them focus on differences rather than similarities between affective and cognitive processes. This research orientation is increasingly challenged by the widespread and successful use of cognitive research paradigms in the study of affect and emotion—a challenge with far-reaching implications. Where and on what basis should theorists draw the line between cognition and emotion, and when is it useful to do so? (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Common valence coding in action and evaluation: Affective blindness towards response-compatible stimuli.Andreas B. Eder & Karl Christoph Klauer - 2007 - Cognition and Emotion 21 (6):1297-1322.
    A common coding account of bidirectional evaluation–behaviour interactions proposes that evaluative attributes of stimuli and responses are coded in a common representational format. This assumption was tested in two experiments that required evaluations of positive and negative stimuli during the generation of a positively or negatively charged motor response. The results of both experiments revealed a reduced evaluative sensitivity (d′) towards response-compatible stimulus valences. This action–valence blindness supports the notion of a common valence coding in action and evaluation.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Revisiting the affective Simon effect.Katia Duscherer, Daniel Holender & Esther Molenaar - 2008 - Cognition and Emotion 22 (2):193-217.
  • Adult attachment and attentional inhibition of interpersonal stimuli.Marieke Dewitte - 2011 - Cognition and Emotion 25 (4):612-625.
  • Affective priming of semantic categorisation responses.Jan De Houwer, Dirk Hermans, Klaus Rothermund & Dirk Wentura - 2002 - Cognition and Emotion 16 (5):643-666.
  • On the (un-) controllability of affective priming: Strategic manipulation is feasible but can possibly be prevented.Juliane Degner - 2009 - Cognition and Emotion 23 (2):327-354.
  • Processing of “unattended” threat-related information: Role of emotional content and context.Manuel G. Calvo, M. Dolores Castillo & Luis J. Fuentes - 2006 - Cognition and Emotion 20 (8):1049-1074.
  • The artful mind meets art history: Toward a psycho-historical framework for the science of art appreciation.Nicolas J. Bullot & Rolf Reber - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (2):123-137.
    Research seeking a scientific foundation for the theory of art appreciation has raised controversies at the intersection of the social and cognitive sciences. Though equally relevant to a scientific inquiry into art appreciation, psychological and historical approaches to art developed independently and lack a common core of theoretical principles. Historicists argue that psychological and brain sciences ignore the fact that artworks are artifacts produced and appreciated in the context of unique historical situations and artistic intentions. After revealing flaws in the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   53 citations  
  • The impact of open and closed mindsets on evaluative priming.Theodore Alexopoulos, Klaus Fiedler & Peter Freytag - 2012 - Cognition and Emotion 26 (6):978-994.
  • Higher order influences on evaluative priming: Processing styles moderate congruity effects.Theodore Alexopoulos, Aurore Lemonnier & Klaus Fiedler - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 31 (1):57-68.
  • Pleasure.Leonard D. Katz - 2008 - Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.
    Pleasure, in the inclusive usages most important in moral psychology, ethical theory, and the studies of mind, includes all joy and gladness — all our feeling good, or happy. It is often contrasted with similarly inclusive pain, or suffering, which is similarly thought of as including all our feeling bad. Contemporary psychology similarly distinguishes between positive affect and negative affect.[1..
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  • Implicit association test: Validity debates.Anthony Greenwald - manuscript
    Note posted 9 Jun 08 : Modifications made today include a new section on predictive validity, and addition of recently published article and in in-press article, both by Nosek & Hansen, under the "CULTURE VS. PERSON" heading, which replaces a previously listed unpublished ms. of theirs. I continue to encourage all interested to send material that they are willing to be included on this page. Please also to let me know about errors, including faulty links.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark