Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science.Andy Clark - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (3):181-204.
    Brains, it has recently been argued, are essentially prediction machines. They are bundles of cells that support perception and action by constantly attempting to match incoming sensory inputs with top-down expectations or predictions. This is achieved using a hierarchical generative model that aims to minimize prediction error within a bidirectional cascade of cortical processing. Such accounts offer a unifying model of perception and action, illuminate the functional role of attention, and may neatly capture the special contribution of cortical processing to (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   725 citations  
  • Cortical connections and parallel processing: Structure and function.Dana H. Ballard - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):67-90.
    The cerebral cortex is a rich and diverse structure that is the basis of intelligent behavior. One of the deepest mysteries of the function of cortex is that neural processing times are only about one hundred times as fast as the fastest response times for complex behavior. At the very least, this would seem to indicate that the cortex does massive amounts of parallel computation.This paper explores the hypothesis that an important part of the cortex can be modeled as a (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   106 citations  
  • What's in the term connectionist?.Christof Koch - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):100-101.
  • Value units make the right connections.Dana H. Ballard - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):107-120.
    The cerebral cortex is a rich and diverse structure that is the basis of intelligent behavior. One of the deepest mysteries of the function of cortex is that neural processing times are only about one hundred times as fast as the fastest response times for complex behavior. At the very least, this would seem to indicate that the cortex does massive amounts of parallel computation.This paper explores the hypothesis that an important part of the cortex can be modeled as a (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Putting together connectionism – again.Paul Smolensky - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):59-74.
    A set of hypotheses is formulated for a connectionist approach to cognitive modeling. These hypotheses are shown to be incompatible with the hypotheses underlying traditional cognitive models. The connectionist models considered are massively parallel numerical computational systems that are a kind of continuous dynamical system. The numerical variables in the system correspond semantically to fine-grained features below the level of the concepts consciously used to describe the task domain. The level of analysis is intermediate between those of symbolic cognitive models (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Toward a theory of human memory: Data structures and access processes.Michael S. Humphreys, Janet Wiles & Simon Dennis - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):655-667.
    Starting from Marr's ideas about levels of explanation, a theory of the data structures and access processes in human memory is demonstrated on 10 tasks. Functional characteristics of human memory are captured implementation-independently. Our theory generates a multidimensional task classification subsuming existing classifications such as the distinction between tasks that are implicit versus explicit, data driven versus conceptually driven, and simple associative (two-way bindings) versus higher order (threeway bindings), providing a broad basis for new experiments. The formal language clarifies the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   69 citations  
  • A predictive coding model of the N400.Samer Nour Eddine, Trevor Brothers, Lin Wang, Michael Spratling & Gina R. Kuperberg - 2024 - Cognition 246 (C):105755.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The locus of the effects of sentential-semantic context in spoken-word processing.Pienie Zwitserlood - 1989 - Cognition 32 (1):25-64.
  • Modularity need not imply locality: Damaged modules can have nonlocal effects.Edgar Zurif & David Swinney - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):89-90.
  • Does connectionism suffice?Steven W. Zucker - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):301-302.
  • Modeling language and cognition with deep unsupervised learning: a tutorial overview.Marco Zorzi, Alberto Testolin & Ivilin P. Stoianov - 2013 - Frontiers in Psychology 4.
  • Developmental dyslexia and the dual route model of reading: Simulating individual differences and subtypes.Johannes C. Ziegler, Caroline Castel, Catherine Pech-Georgel, Florence George, F.-Xavier Alario & Conrad Perry - 2008 - Cognition 107 (1):151-178.
  • A Comparative Study of Three Measurement Methods of Chinese Character Recognition for L2 Chinese Learners.Haiwei Zhang, Sun-A. Kim & Xueyan Zhang - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Measuring Chinese character recognition ability is essential in research on character learning among learners of Chinese as a second language. Three methods are typically used to evaluate character recognition competence by investigating the following properties of a given character: pronunciation, meaning, and pronunciation and meaning. However, no study has explored the similar or dissimilar outcomes that these three measurements might yield. The current study examined this issue by testing 162 CSL learners with various L1 backgrounds and Chinese proficiency levels. Participants' (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • What counts as local?Andrew W. Young - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):88-89.
  • The reality of the symbolic and subsymbolic systems.Andrew Woodfield & Adam Morton - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):58-58.
  • A review of the literature with and without awareness. [REVIEW]George Wolford - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):49-50.
  • On the Interaction of Theory and Data in Concept Learning.Edward J. Wisniewski & Douglas L. Medin - 1994 - Cognitive Science 18 (2):221-281.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   36 citations  
  • Influence of Suboptimally and Optimally Presented Affective Pictures and Words on Consumption-Related Behavior.Piotr Winkielman & Yekaterina Gogolushko - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • On the creation of classification systems of memory.Daniel B. Willingham - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):426-427.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Does cognitive neuropsychology have a future?J. T. L. Wilson - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):456-457.
  • Classical conditioning and the placebo effect.Ian Wickram - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):160-161.
  • Classical conditioning: A manifestation of Bayesian neural learning.James Christopher Westland & Manfred Kochen - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):160-160.
  • Cognition as self–organizing process.Gerhard Werner - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):183-183.
  • Parallel, cascaded, interactive processing of words during sentence reading.Yun Wen, Joshua Snell & Jonathan Grainger - 2019 - Cognition 189 (C):221-226.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Connectionist learning and the challenge of real environments.Mark Weaver & Stephen Kaplan - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):510-511.
  • The localization/distribution distinction in neuropsychology is related to the isomorphism/multiple meaning distinction in cell electrophysiology.Gerald S. Wasserman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):87-88.
  • Opposite size illusions for inverted faces and letters.Eamonn Walsh, Carolina Moreira & Matthew R. Longo - 2024 - Cognition 245 (C):105733.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A self-organized sentence processing theory of gradience: The case of islands.Sandra Villata & Whitney Tabor - 2022 - Cognition 222 (C):104943.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Smolensky's theory of mind.Paul F. M. J. Verschure - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):407-407.
  • The Word Composite Effect Depends on Abstract Lexical Representations But Not Surface Features Like Case and Font.Paulo Ventura, Tânia Fernandes, Isabel Leite, Vítor B. Almeida, Inês Casqueiro & Alan C.-N. Wong - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Words with and without internal structure: What determines the nature of orthographic and morphological processing?Hadas Velan & Ram Frost - 2011 - Cognition 118 (2):141-156.
    No categories
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Side flankers produce less crowding, but only for letters.Dušan Vejnović & Sunčica Zdravković - 2015 - Cognition 143 (C):217-227.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Orthographic Activation in L2 Spoken Word Recognition Depends on Proficiency: Evidence from Eye-Tracking.Outi Veivo, Juhani Järvikivi, Vincent Porretta & Jukka Hyönä - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • On observing emergent properties and their compositions.Francisco T. Varela & Vicente Sanchez-Leighton - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):401-402.
  • Criteria for the Design and Evaluation of Cognitive Architectures.Sashank Varma - 2011 - Cognitive Science 35 (7):1329-1351.
    Cognitive architectures are unified theories of cognition that take the form of computational formalisms. They support computational models that collectively account for large numbers of empirical regularities using small numbers of computational mechanisms. Empirical coverage and parsimony are the most prominent criteria by which architectures are designed and evaluated, but they are not the only ones. This paper considers three additional criteria that have been comparatively undertheorized. (a) Successful architectures possess subjective and intersubjective meaning, making cognition comprehensible to individual cognitive (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • What do double dissociations prove?G. Van Orden - 2001 - Cognitive Science 25 (1):111-172.
    Brain damage may doubly dissociate cognitive modules, but the practice of revealing dissociations is predicated on modularity being true (T. Shallice, 1988). This article questions the utility of assuming modularity, as it examines a paradigmatic double dissociation of reading modules. Reading modules illustrate two general problems. First, modularity fails to converge on a fixed set of exclusionary criteria that define pure cases. As a consequence, competing modular theories force perennial quests for purer cases, which simply perpetuates growth in the list (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   44 citations  
  • The symbolic brain or the invisible hand?René van Hezewijk & Edward H. F. de Haan - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):85-86.
  • The emergence of frequency effects in eye movements.Polina M. Vanyukov, Tessa Warren, Mark E. Wheeler & Erik D. Reichle - 2012 - Cognition 123 (1):185-189.
    Direct download (11 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The dynamical hypothesis in cognitive science.Tim van Gelder - 1998 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 (5):615-28.
    According to the dominant computational approach in cognitive science, cognitive agents are digital computers; according to the alternative approach, they are dynamical systems. This target article attempts to articulate and support the dynamical hypothesis. The dynamical hypothesis has two major components: the nature hypothesis (cognitive agents are dynamical systems) and the knowledge hypothesis (cognitive agents can be understood dynamically). A wide range of objections to this hypothesis can be rebutted. The conclusion is that cognitive systems may well be dynamical systems, (...)
    Direct download (11 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   207 citations  
  • The architecture of cognition.Kurt VanLehn - 1986 - Artificial Intelligence 28 (2):235-240.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Playing Flourens to Fodor's Gall.Tim van Gelder - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):84-84.
  • Prosopagnosia, conscious awareness and the interactive brain.Robert Van Gulick - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):84-85.
  • Marr versus Marr: On the notion of levels.Frank van der Velde, Gezinus Wolters & A. H. C. van der Heijden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):681-682.
  • Has the case been made against the ecumenical view of connectionism?Robert Van Gulick - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):57-58.
  • Connectionist models learn what?Timothy van Gelder - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):509-510.
  • Acquiring Contextualized Concepts: A Connectionist Approach.Saskia van Dantzig, Antonino Raffone & Bernhard Hommel - 2011 - Cognitive Science 35 (6):1162-1189.
    Conceptual knowledge is acquired through recurrent experiences, by extracting statistical regularities at different levels of granularity. At a fine level, patterns of feature co-occurrence are categorized into objects. At a coarser level, patterns of concept co-occurrence are categorized into contexts. We present and test CONCAT, a connectionist model that simultaneously learns to categorize objects and contexts. The model contains two hierarchically organized CALM modules (Murre, Phaf, & Wolters, 1992). The first module, the Object Module, forms object representations based on co-occurrences (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Automatic and controlled semantic processing: A masked prime-task effect.B. Valdés, A. Catena & P. Marí-Beffa - 2005 - Consciousness and Cognition 14 (2):278-295.
    A classical definition of automaticity establishes that automatic processing occurs without attention or consciousness, and cannot be controlled. Previous studies have demonstrated that semantic priming can be reduced if attention is directed to a low-level of analysis. This finding suggests that semantic processing is not automatic since it can be controlled. In this paper, we present two experiments that demonstrate that semantic processing may occur in the absence of attention and consciousness. A negative semantic priming effect was found when a (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Refuting the unfolding-argument on the irrelevance of causal structure to consciousness.Marius Usher - 2021 - Consciousness and Cognition 95 (C):103212.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Facilitation or inhibition from parafoveal words?Geoffrey Underwood - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):48-49.
  • Interactive processes in word recognition.Geoffrey Underwood - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):727-728.