Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Introspective access to implicit shifts of attention.Gabriel Reyes & Jérôme Sackur - 2017 - Consciousness and Cognition 48:11-20.
  • Introspection during visual search.Gabriel Reyes & Jérôme Sackur - 2014 - Consciousness and Cognition 29:212-229.
  • Metacognitive blindness in temporal selection during the deployment of spatial attention.Samuel Recht, Vincent de Gardelle & Pascal Mamassian - 2021 - Cognition 216 (C):104864.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Subjective report of eye fixations during serial search.Sébastien Marti, Laurie Bayet & Stanislas Dehaene - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 33:1-15.
  • Would you follow your own route description? Cognitive strategies in urban route planning.Christoph Hölscher, Thora Tenbrink & Jan M. Wiener - 2011 - Cognition 121 (2):228-247.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Possibilities and limits of mind-reading: A neurophilosophical perspective.Kathinka Evers & Mariano Sigman - 2013 - Consciousness and Cognition 22 (3):887-897.
    Access to other minds once presupposed other individuals’ expressions and narrations. Today, several methods have been developed which can measure brain states relevant for assessments of mental states without 1st person overt external behavior or speech. Functional magnetic resonance imaging and trace conditioning are used clinically to identify patterns of activity in the brain that suggest the presence of consciousness in people suffering from severe consciousness disorders and methods to communicate cerebrally with patients who are motorically unable to communicate. The (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Toward a Metacognitive Account of Cognitive Offloading.Timothy L. Dunn & Evan F. Risko - 2016 - Cognitive Science 40 (5):1080-1127.
    Individuals frequently make use of the body and environment when engaged in a cognitive task. For example, individuals will often spontaneously physically rotate when faced with rotated objects, such as an array of words, to putatively offload the performance costs associated with stimulus rotation. We looked to further examine this idea by independently manipulating the costs associated with both word rotation and array frame rotation. Surprisingly, we found that individuals’ patterns of spontaneous physical rotations did not follow patterns of performance (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Sense of control depends on fluency of action selection, not motor performance.Valerian Chambon & Patrick Haggard - 2012 - Cognition 125 (3):441-451.
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  • Introspective reports of reaction times in dual-tasks reflect experienced difficulty rather than timing of cognitive processes.Donna Bryce & Daniel Bratzke - 2014 - Consciousness and Cognition 27:254-267.
  • Distorted subjective reports of stimulus onsets under dual-task conditions: Delayed conscious perception or estimation bias?Daniel Bratzke, Donna Bryce & Tanja Seifried-Dübon - 2014 - Consciousness and Cognition 30:36-47.
  • Biological Stress Reactivity and Introspective Sensitivity: An Exploratory Study.Mauricio Barrientos, Leonel Tapia, Jaime R. Silva & Gabriel Reyes - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Validating and calibrating first-and second-person methods in the science of consciousness.T. Froese, C. Gould & A. K. Seth - 2011 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 18 (2):38.