Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Skill acquisition in music performance: relations between planning and temporal control.Carolyn Drake & Caroline Palmer - 2000 - Cognition 74 (1):1-32.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • A Dynamic Network Approach to the Study of Syntax.Holger Diessel - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Usage-based linguists and psychologists have produced a large body of empirical results suggesting that linguistic structure is derived from language use. However, while researchers agree that these results characterize grammar as an emergent phenomenon, there is no consensus among usage-based scholars as to how the various results can be explained and integrated into an explicit theory or model. Building on network theory, the current paper outlines a structured network approach to the study of grammar in which the core concepts of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Self-regulation and the hypothesis of experience-based selection: Investigating indirect conscious control.Derek C. Dorris - 2009 - Consciousness and Cognition 18 (3):740-753.
  • Language production and serial order: A functional analysis and a model.Gary S. Dell, Lisa K. Burger & William R. Svec - 1997 - Psychological Review 104 (1):123-147.
  • On How Definitions of Habits Can Complicate Habit Research.Jan De Houwer - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
  • Processes Versus Representations: Cognitive Control as Emergent, Yet Componential.Eddy J. Davelaar - 2011 - Topics in Cognitive Science 3 (2):247-252.
    In this commentary, I focus on the difference between processes and representations and how this distinction relates to the question of what is controlled. Despite some views that task switching is a prototypical control process, the analysis concludes that task switching depends on the task goal representation and that control processes are there to prevent goal representations from disintegrating. Over time, these processes become obsolete, leaving behind a representation that automatically controls task performance. The distinction between processes and representations relates (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Implementing flexibility in automaticity: Evidence from context-specific implicit sequence learning.Maria C. D’Angelo, Bruce Milliken, Luis Jiménez & Juan Lupiáñez - 2013 - Consciousness and Cognition 22 (1):64-81.
    Attention is often dichotomized into controlled vs. automatic processing, where controlled processing is slow, flexible, and intentional, and automatic processing is fast, inflexible, and unintentional. In contrast to this strict dichotomy, there is mounting evidence for context-specific processes that are engaged rapidly yet are also flexible. In the present study we extend this idea to the domain of implicit learning to examine whether flexibility in automatic processes can be implemented through the reliance on contextual features. Across three experiments we show (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Changes in Cue Configuration Reduce the Impact of Interfering Information in a Predictive Learning Task.Carmelo P. Cubillas, Miguel A. Vadillo & Helena Matute - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A dynamic approach to recognition memory.Gregory E. Cox & Richard M. Shiffrin - 2017 - Psychological Review 124 (6):795-860.
    No categories
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Flow Experiences During Visuomotor Skill Acquisition Reflect Deviation From a Power-Law Learning Curve, but Not Overall Level of Skill.Benjamin Ultan Cowley, Jussi Palomäki, Tuisku Tammi, Roosa Frantsi, Ville-Pekka Inkilä, Noora Lehtonen, Pasi Pölönen, Juha Vepsäläinen & Otto Lappi - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Where is the classic interference theory for sleep and memory?Anton Coenen - 2005 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (1):67-68.
    Walker's target article proposes a refinement of the well known two-stage model of memory formation to explain the positive effects of sleep on consolidation. After a first stage in which a labile memory representation is formed, a further stabilisation of the memory trace takes place in the second stage, which is dependent on (REM) sleep. Walker has refined the latter stage into a stage in which a consolidation-based enhancement occurs. It is not completely clear what consolidation-based enhancement implies and how (...)
    Direct download (10 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Make Gestures to Learn: Reproducing Gestures Improves the Learning of Anatomical Knowledge More than Just Seeing Gestures.Mélaine Cherdieu, Olivier Palombi, Silvain Gerber, Jocelyne Troccaz & Amélie Rochet-Capellan - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The paradox of social interaction: Shared intentionality, we-reasoning, and virtual bargaining.Nick Chater, Hossam Zeitoun & Tigran Melkonyan - 2022 - Psychological Review 129 (3):415-437.
    Social interaction is both ubiquitous and central to understanding human behavior. Such interactions depend, we argue, on shared intentionality: the parties must form a common understanding of an ambiguous interaction. Yet how can shared intentionality arise? Many well-known accounts of social cognition, including those involving “mind-reading,” typically fall into circularity and/or regress. For example, A’s beliefs and behavior may depend on her prediction of B’s beliefs and behavior, but B’s beliefs and behavior depend in turn on her prediction of A’s (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Theories or fragments?Nick Chater & Mike Oaksford - 2017 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 40.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rational and mechanistic perspectives on reinforcement learning.Nick Chater - 2009 - Cognition 113 (3):350-364.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Strengths and weaknesses of reflection as a guide to action: pressure assails performance in multiple ways.Thomas H. Carr - 2015 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 14 (2):227-252.
    The current status of Beilock and Carr's "execution focus" theory of choking under pressure in performance of a sensorimotor skill is reviewed and assessed, mainly from the perspective of cognitive psychology, and put into the context of a wider range of issues, attempting to take philosophical analysis into account. These issues include other kinds of skills, pre-performance practice, post-performance evaluation and repair, and integrating new and creative achievements into repertoires of heavily practiced routines. The focus is on variation in the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • In Support of a Distinction between Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Control: A Review of the Literature on Proportion Congruent Effects. [REVIEW]Julie M. Bugg & Matthew J. C. Crump - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
  • Learning to explore the structure of kinematic objects in a virtual environment.Marcus Buckmann, Robert Gaschler, Sebastian Höfer, Dennis Loeben, Peter A. Frensch & Oliver Brock - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Motivating inhibition – reward prospect speeds up response cancellation.Carsten N. Boehler, Jens-Max Hopf, Christian M. Stoppel & Ruth M. Krebs - 2012 - Cognition 125 (3):498-503.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Quantum cognition and bounded rationality.Reinhard Blutner & Peter Beim Graben - 2016 - Synthese 193 (10).
    We consider several puzzles of bounded rationality. These include the Allais- and Ellsberg paradox, the disjunction effect, and related puzzles. We argue that the present account of quantum cognition—taking quantum probabilities rather than classical probabilities—can give a more systematic description of these puzzles than the alternate treatments in the traditional frameworks of bounded rationality. Unfortunately, the quantum probabilistic treatment does not always provide a deeper understanding and a true explanation of these puzzles. One reason is that quantum approaches introduce additional (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Cognitive and Motor Learning in Internally-Guided Motor Skills.Krishn Bera, Anuj Shukla & Raju S. Bapi - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Several canonical experimental paradigms have been proposed to study the typical behavioral phenomenon and the nature of learning in sequential keypress tasks. A characteristic feature of most paradigms is that they are representative of externally-specified sequencing—motor tasks where the environment or task paradigm extrinsically provides the sequence of stimuli, i.e., the responses are stimulus-driven. Previous studies utilizing such canonical paradigms have largely overlooked the learning behaviors in a more realistic class of motor tasks that involve internally-guided sequencing—where the sequence of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the fragility of skilled performance: What governs choking under pressure?Sian L. Beilock & Thomas H. Carr - 2001 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130 (4):701.
  • Varieties of consciousness.Paolo Bartolomeo & Gianfranco Dalla Barba - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (3):331-332.
    In agreement with some of the ideas expressed by Perruchet & Vinter (P&V), we believe that some phenomena hitherto attributed to processing may in fact reflect a fundamental distinction between direct and reflexive forms of consciousness. This dichotomy, developed by the phenomenological tradition, is substantiated by examples coming from experimental psychology and lesion neuropsychology.
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Perceptual symbol systems.Lawrence W. Barsalou - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (4):577-660.
    Prior to the twentieth century, theories of knowledge were inherently perceptual. Since then, developments in logic, statis- tics, and programming languages have inspired amodal theories that rest on principles fundamentally different from those underlying perception. In addition, perceptual approaches have become widely viewed as untenable because they are assumed to implement record- ing systems, not conceptual systems. A perceptual theory of knowledge is developed here in the context of current cognitive science and neuroscience. During perceptual experience, association areas in the (...)
    Direct download (10 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   722 citations  
  • Automaticity in social-cognitive processes.John A. Bargh, Kay L. Schwader, Sarah E. Hailey, Rebecca L. Dyer & Erica J. Boothby - 2012 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16 (12):593-605.
  • ADAPT: A Developmental, Asemantic, and Procedural Model for Transcoding From Verbal to Arabic Numerals.Pierre Barrouillet, Valérie Camos, Pierre Perruchet & Xavier Seron - 2004 - Psychological Review 111 (2):368-394.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • The Relationship Between Reading Fluency and Arithmetic Fact Fluency and Their Shared Cognitive Skills: A Developmental Perspective.Reut Balhinez & Shelley Shaul - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Why do young infants fail to search for hidden objects?Renée Baillargeon, Marcia Graber, Julia Devos & James Black - 1990 - Cognition 36 (3):255-284.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy.Edward Awh, Artem V. Belopolsky & Jan Theeuwes - 2012 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16 (8):437.
    Prominent models of attentional control assert a dichotomy between top-down and bottom-up control, with the former determined by current selection goals and the latter determined by physical salience. This theoretical dichotomy, however, fails to explain a growing number of cases in which neither current goals nor physical salience can account for strong selection biases. For example, equally salient stimuli associated with reward can capture attention, even when this contradicts current selection goals. Thus, although 'top-down' sources of bias are sometimes defined (...)
    Direct download (11 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   93 citations  
  • A neurobiological theory of automaticity in perceptual categorization.F. Gregory Ashby, John M. Ennis & Brian J. Spiering - 2007 - Psychological Review 114 (3):632-656.
  • Five-Year-Olds’ Systematic Errors in Second-Order False Belief Tasks Are Due to First-Order Theory of Mind Strategy Selection: A Computational Modeling Study.Burcu Arslan, Niels A. Taatgen & Rineke Verbrugge - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
  • The Relationship Between Uncertainty and Affect.Eric C. Anderson, R. Nicholas Carleton, Michael Diefenbach & Paul K. J. Han - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10:469966.
    Uncertainty and affect are fundamental and interrelated aspects of the human condition. Uncertainty is often associated with negative affect, but in some circumstances it is associated with positive affect. In this paper, we review different explanations for the varying relationship between uncertainty and affect. We identify “mental simulation” as a key process that links uncertainty to affective states. We suggest that people have a propensity to simulate negative outcomes, which results in a propensity towards negative affective responses to uncertainty. We (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Spanning seven orders of magnitude: a challenge for cognitive modeling.John R. Anderson - 2002 - Cognitive Science 26 (1):85-112.
    Much of cognitive psychology focuses on effects measured in tens of milliseconds while significant educational outcomes take tens of hours to achieve. The task of bridging this gap is analyzed in terms of Newell's (1990) bands of cognition—the Biological, Cognitive, Rational, and Social Bands. The 10 millisecond effects reside in his Biological Band while the significant learning outcomes reside in his Social Band. The paper assesses three theses: The Decomposition Thesis claims that learning occurring at the Social Band can be (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • Skill Acquisition and the LISP Tutor.John R. Anderson, Frederick G. Conrad & Albert T. Corbett - 1989 - Cognitive Science 13 (4):467-505.
    An analysis of student learning with the LISP tutor indicates that while LISP is complex, learning it is simple. The key to factoring out the complexity of LISP is to monitor the learning of the 500 productions in the LISP tutor which describe the programming skill. The learning of these productions follows the power‐law learning curve typical of skill acquisition. There is transfer from other programming experience to the extent that this programming experience involves the same productions. Subjects appear to (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Memory for goals: an activation‐based model.Erik M. Altmann & J. Gregory Trafton - 2002 - Cognitive Science 26 (1):39-83.
    Goal‐directed cognition is often discussed in terms of specialized memory structures like the “goal stack.” The goal‐activation model presented here analyzes goal‐directed cognition in terms of the general memory constructs of activation and associative priming. The model embodies three predictive constraints: (1) the interference level, which arises from residual memory for old goals; (1) the strengthening constraint, which makes predictions about time to encode a new goal; and (3) the priming constraint, which makes predictions about the role of cues in (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • Episodic Indexing: A Model of Memory for Attention Events.Erik M. Altmann & Bonnie E. John - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (2):117-156.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Unconscious symmetrical inferences: A role of consciousness in event integration.Diego Alonso, Luis J. Fuentes & Bernhard Hommel - 2006 - Consciousness and Cognition 15 (2):386-396.
    Explicit and implicit learning have been attributed to different learning processes that create different types of knowledge structures. Consistent with that claim, our study provides evidence that people integrate stimulus events differently when consciously aware versus unaware of the relationship between the events. In a first, acquisition phase participants sorted words into two categories , which were fully predicted by task-irrelevant primes—the labels of two other, semantically unrelated categories . In a second, test phase participants performed a lexical decision task, (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Models, Mechanisms, and Animal Minds.Colin Allen - 2014 - Southern Journal of Philosophy 52 (S1):75-97.
    In this paper, I describe grounds for dissatisfaction with certain aspects of the sciences of animal cognition and argue that a turn toward mathematical modeling of animal cognition is warranted. I consider some objections to this call and argue that the implications of such a turn are not as drastic for ordinary, commonsense understanding of animal minds as they might seem.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Perseverative responding in a violation-of-expectation task in 6.5-month-old infants.Andréa Aguiar & Renée Baillargeon - 2003 - Cognition 88 (3):277-316.
  • Control of automated behavior: insights from the discrete sequence production task.Elger L. Abrahamse, Marit F. L. Ruitenberg, Elian de Kleine & Willem B. Verwey - 2013 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7.
  • Mental imagery.Nigel J. T. Thomas - 2001 - Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.
    Mental imagery (varieties of which are sometimes colloquially refered to as “visualizing,” “seeing in the mind's eye,” “hearing in the head,” “imagining the feel of,” etc.) is quasi-perceptual experience; it resembles perceptual experience, but occurs in the absence of the appropriate external stimuli. It is also generally understood to bear intentionality (i.e., mental images are always images of something or other), and thereby to function as a form of mental representation. Traditionally, visual mental imagery, the most discussed variety, was thought (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • References.John Bengson & Marc A. Moffett - 2011 - In Knowing How: Essays on Knowledge, Mind, and Action. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 361-386.
    This compilation of references includes all references for the knowledge-how chapters included in Bengson & Moffett's edited volume. The volume and the compilation of references may serve as a good starting point for people who are unfamiliar with the philosophical literature on knowledge-how.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Cognitive Control: Dynamic, Sustained, and Voluntary Influences.MaryBeth Knight - unknown
    The cost of incongruent stimuli is reduced when conflict is expected. This series of experiments tested whether this improved performance is due to repetition priming or to enhanced cognitive control. Using a paradigm in which Word and Number Stroop alternated every trial, Experiment 1 assessed dynamic trial-to-trial changes. Incongruent trials led to task-specific reduction of conflict (trial n ϩ 2) without cross-task modulation (trial n ϩ 1), but this was fully explained by repetition priming. In contrast, an increased ratio of (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Increased practice with 'set'problems hinders performance on the water jar task.Noelle M. Crooks, Nicole M. McNeil, N. Taatgen & H. Van Rijn - 2009 - In N. A. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A plea for methodological dualism and multi-explanation framework in psychology.Sam S. Rakover - 2011 - Behavior and Philosophy 39:17-43.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • What is consciousness for?Lee Pierson & Monroe Trout - manuscript
    What is Consciousness For? Lee Pierson and Monroe Trout Copyright © 2005 Abstract: The answer to the title question is, in a word, volition. Our hypothesis is that the ultimate adaptive function of consciousness is to make volitional movement possible. All conscious processes exist to subserve that ultimate function. Thus, we believe that all conscious organisms possess at least some volitional capability. Consciousness makes volitional attention possible; volitional attention, in turn, makes volitional movement possible. There is, as far as we (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Thinking as a production system.Marsha C. Lovett & John R. Anderson - 2005 - In K. Holyoak & B. Morrison (eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning. Cambridge University Press. pp. 401--429.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Re-Examining the Role of Consistency: The Cornerstone, not Simply an Important Factor.Patrice Terrier - 1998 - PSYCHE: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Research On Consciousness 4.
    Despite the important role of the consistency concept in various theoretical frameworks of memory research and its influence on practical investigations it remains unclear as to whether consistency has been firmly grounded as a explanatory factor. Consistency does not determine either a cognitive load or the development of automaticity. However, it does explain the nature of empirical facts that are subsumed by these terms. Consistency is not a psychological factor involved in many important and highly related topics of consciousness research (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation