Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Putting together connectionism – again.Paul Smolensky - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):59-74.
    A set of hypotheses is formulated for a connectionist approach to cognitive modeling. These hypotheses are shown to be incompatible with the hypotheses underlying traditional cognitive models. The connectionist models considered are massively parallel numerical computational systems that are a kind of continuous dynamical system. The numerical variables in the system correspond semantically to fine-grained features below the level of the concepts consciously used to describe the task domain. The level of analysis is intermediate between those of symbolic cognitive models (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • The reality of the symbolic and subsymbolic systems.Andrew Woodfield & Adam Morton - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):58-58.
  • Has the case been made against the ecumenical view of connectionism?Robert Van Gulick - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):57-58.
  • On the proper treatment of thermostats.David S. Touretzky - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):55-56.
    A set of hypotheses is formulated for a connectionist approach to cognitive modeling. These hypotheses are shown to be incompatible with the hypotheses underlying traditional cognitive models. The connectionist models considered are massively parallel numerical computational systems that are a kind of continuous dynamical system. The numerical variables in the system correspond semantically to fine-grained features below the level of the concepts consciously used to describe the task domain. The level of analysis is intermediate between those of symbolic cognitive models (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cognitive Load During Problem Solving: Effects on Learning.John Sweller - 1988 - Cognitive Science 12 (2):257-285.
    Considerable evidence indicates that domain specific knowledge in the form of schemas is the primary factor distinguishing experts from novices in problem‐solving skill. Evidence that conventional problem‐solving activity is not effective in schema acquisition is also accumulating. It is suggested that a major reason for the ineffectiveness of problem solving as a learning device, is that the cognitive processes required by the two activities overlap insufficiently, and that conventional problem solving in the form of means‐ends analysis requires a relatively large (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   35 citations  
  • Cognitive Load During Problem Solving: Effects on Learning.John Sweller - 1988 - Cognitive Science 12 (2):257-285.
    Considerable evidence indicates that domain specific knowledge in the form of schemas is the primary factor distinguishing experts from novices in problem‐solving skill. Evidence that conventional problem‐solving activity is not effective in schema acquisition is also accumulating. It is suggested that a major reason for the ineffectiveness of problem solving as a learning device, is that the cognitive processes required by the two activities overlap insufficiently, and that conventional problem solving in the form of means‐ends analysis requires a relatively large (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   78 citations  
  • From data to dynamics: The use of multiple levels of analysis.Gregory O. Stone - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):54-55.
  • From connectionism to eliminativism.Stephen P. Stich - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):53-54.
  • Components of human intelligence.Robert J. Sternberg - 1983 - Cognition 15 (1-3):1-48.
  • On the proper treatment of connectionism.Paul Smolensky - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):1-23.
    A set of hypotheses is formulated for a connectionist approach to cognitive modeling. These hypotheses are shown to be incompatible with the hypotheses underlying traditional cognitive models. The connectionist models considered are massively parallel numerical computational systems that are a kind of continuous dynamical system. The numerical variables in the system correspond semantically to fine-grained features below the level of the concepts consciously used to describe the task domain. The level of analysis is intermediate between those of symbolic cognitive models (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   741 citations  
  • How fully should connectionism be activated? Two sources of excitation and one of inhibition.Roger N. Shepard - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):52-52.
  • Learning in mathematically-based domains: Understanding and generalizing obstacle cancellations.Jude W. Shavlik & Gerald F. DeJong - 1990 - Artificial Intelligence 45 (1-2):1-45.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Herbert A. Simon as a cyborg scientist.Esther-Mirjam Sent - 2000 - Perspectives on Science 8 (4):380-406.
    : This paper discusses how Herbert Simon's initial interest in decision making became transformed into a focus on understanding human problem solving in response to the concrete conditions of the Cold War and the practical goals of the military. In particular, it suggests a connection between the seachange in Simon's interest and his shift in patronage. As a result, Simon is portrayed as a component of the scientific-military World War II cyborg that further evolved during the Cold War. Moving from (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Structure and controlling subsymbolic processing.Walter Schneider - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):51-52.
  • Making the connections.Jay G. Rueckl - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):50-51.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sanity surrounded by madness.Georges Rey - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):48-50.
  • Expert and novice intuitive judgments about animal behavior.Michael J. Renner & Catherine Hackett Renner - 1993 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 31 (6):551-552.
  • A two-dimensional array of models of cognitive function.Gardner C. Quarton - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):48-48.
  • Subsymbols aren't much good outside of a symbol-processing architecture.Alan Prince & Steven Pinker - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):46-47.
  • Should physicists preach what they practice?Nancy J. Nersessian - 1995 - Science & Education 4 (3):203-226.
  • Connections among connections.R. J. Nelson - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):45-46.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • In defence of neurons.Chris Mortensen - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):44-45.
  • Epistemological challenges for connectionism.John McCarthy - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):44-44.
  • Symbols, subsymbols, neurons.William G. Lycan - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):43-44.
  • Connectionism in the golden age of cognitive science.Dan Lloyd - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):42-43.
  • Can this treatment raise the dead?Robert K. Lindsay - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):41-42.
  • Physics, cognition, and connectionism: An interdisciplinary alchemy.Wendy G. Lehnert - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):40-41.
  • Strategy shifts and expertise in solving transformation rule problems.Vanessa J. Clarke Koen Lamberts - 1997 - Thinking and Reasoning 3 (4):271 – 290.
    The acquisition of expertise in formal problem solving has been assumed to involve either a shift from backwards to forwards inference, or a shift from unguided to guided forwards inference. In a longitudinal study, the acquisition of formal problem-solving expertise was investigated. Participants were tested as novices before undertaking controlled practice in the problem domain which involved transformation rule problems , and were finally tested as experts. The direction of inference in problem solutions was found to be inadequate to describe (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Smolensky, semantics, and the sensorimotor system.George Lakoff - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):39-40.
  • Abstract Planning and Perceptual Chunks: Elements of Expertise in Geometry.Kenneth R. Koedinger & John R. Anderson - 1990 - Cognitive Science 14 (4):511-550.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Two Theories of Home Heat Control.Willett Kempton - 1986 - Cognitive Science 10 (1):75-90.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Peirce, logic diagrams, and the elementary operations of reasoning.P. N. Johnson-Laird - 2002 - Thinking and Reasoning 8 (1):69 – 95.
    This paper describes Peirce's systems of logic diagrams, focusing on the so-called ''existential'' graphs, which are equivalent to the first-order predicate calculus. It analyses their implications for the nature of mental representations, particularly mental models with which they have many characteristics in common. The graphs are intended to be iconic, i.e., to have a structure analogous to the structure of what they represent. They have emergent logical consequences and a single graph can capture all the different ways in which a (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Functional explanation in mathematics.Matthew Inglis & Juan Pablo Mejía Ramos - 2019 - Synthese 198 (26):6369-6392.
    Mathematical explanations are poorly understood. Although mathematicians seem to regularly suggest that some proofs are explanatory whereas others are not, none of the philosophical accounts of what such claims mean has become widely accepted. In this paper we explore Wilkenfeld’s suggestion that explanations are those sorts of things that generate understanding. By considering a basic model of human cognitive architecture, we suggest that existing accounts of mathematical explanation are all derivable consequences of Wilkenfeld’s ‘functional explanation’ proposal. We therefore argue that (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Some memory, but no mind.Lawrence E. Hunter - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):37-38.
  • Common sense and conceptual halos.Douglas R. Hofstadter - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):35-37.
  • On the obvious treatment of connectionism.Stephen José Hanson - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):38-39.
  • Statistical rationality.Richard M. Golden - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):35-35.
  • Young singles' scripts for a first date.Irene Hanson Frieze & Suzanna Rose - 1989 - Gender and Society 3 (2):258-268.
    Young single women's and men's cognitive scripts for the event “a first date” were examined to determine their content and to test for hypothesized differences in behavioral expectations. Participants were asked to list 20 expected actions involved in a first date for a woman and for a man. High agreement among participants was found for the content and sequence of actions that hypothetically would occur on a first date. The respondents listed a total of 19 different actions for women, and (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Dynamic systems and the “subsymbolic level”.Walter J. Freeman - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):33-34.
  • Connectionism and the study of language.R. Freidin - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):34-35.
  • Modeling Novice‐to‐Expert Shifts in Problem‐Solving Strategy and Knowledge Organization.Renée Elio & Peternela B. Scharf - 1990 - Cognitive Science 14 (4):579-639.
    This research presents a computer model called EUREKA that begins with novice‐like strategies and knowledge organizations for solving physics word problems and acquires features of knowledge organizations and basic approaches that characterize experts in this domain. EUREKA learns a highly interrelated network of problem‐type schemas with associated solution methodologies. Initially, superficial features of the problem statement form the basis for both the problem‐type schemas and the discriminating features that organize them in the P‐MOP (Problem Memory Organization Packet) network. As EUREKA (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The promise and problems of connectionism.Michael G. Dyer - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):32-33.
  • On the proper treatment of Smolensky.Hubert L. Dreyfus & Stuart E. Dreyfus - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):31-32.
  • A provisional model of mathematical problem solving.Dale Dinnel, John A. Glover & Royce R. Ronning - 1984 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 22 (5):459-462.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Some assumptions underlying Smolensky's treatment of connectionism.Eric Dietrich & Chris Fields - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):29-31.
  • The essential opacity of modular systems: Why even connectionism cannot give complete formal accounts of cognition.Marten J. den Uyl - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):56-57.
  • The psychological appeal of connectionism.Denise Dellarosa - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):28-29.
  • Is Smolensky's treatment of connectionism on the level?Carol E. Cleland - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):27-28.
  • Information processing abstractions: The message still counts more than the medium.B. Chandrasekaran, Ashok Goel & Dean Allemang - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):26-27.
  • Mental models: An alternative evaluation of a sensemaking approach to ethics instruction.Meagan E. Brock, Andrew Vert, Vykinta Kligyte, Ethan P. Waples, Sydney T. Sevier & Michael D. Mumford - 2008 - Science and Engineering Ethics 14 (3):449-472.
    In spite of the wide variety of approaches to ethics training it is still debatable which approach has the highest potential to enhance professionals’ integrity. The current effort assesses a novel curriculum that focuses on metacognitive reasoning strategies researchers use when making sense of day-to-day professional practices that have ethical implications. The evaluated trainings effectiveness was assessed by examining five key sensemaking processes, such as framing, emotion regulation, forecasting, self-reflection, and information integration that experts and novices apply in ethical decision-making. (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations