Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. A cognitive theory of graphical and linguistic reasoning: Logic and implementation. Cognitive science.Keith Stenning & Jon Oberlander - 1995 - Cognitive Science 19 (1):97-140.
    We discuss external and internal graphical and linguistic representational systems. We argue that a cognitive theory of peoples' reasoning performance must account for (a) the logical equivalence of inferences expressed in graphical and linguistic form; and (b) the implementational differences that affect facility of inference. Our theory proposes that graphical representations limit abstraction and thereby aid processibility. We discuss the ideas of specificity and abstraction, and their cognitive relevance. Empirical support comes from tasks (i) involving and (ii) not involving the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  • The Modal—Amodal Distinction in the Debate on Conceptual Format.Sabrina Haimovici - 2018 - Philosophies 3 (2):7.
    In this paper, I review the main criteria offered for distinguishing the modal and amodal approaches to conceptual format: the type of input to which the representations respond, the relation they bear to perceptual states, and the specific neural systems to which they belong. I evaluate different interpretations of them and argue that they all face difficulties. I further show that they lead to cross-classifications of certain types of representations, using approximate number representations as an example.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Mental imagery: In search of a theory.Zenon W. Pylyshyn - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (2):157-182.
    It is generally accepted that there is something special about reasoning by using mental images. The question of how it is special, however, has never been satisfactorily spelled out, despite more than thirty years of research in the post-behaviorist tradition. This article considers some of the general motivation for the assumption that entertaining mental images involves inspecting a picture-like object. It sets out a distinction between phenomena attributable to the nature of mind to what is called the cognitive architecture, and (...)
    Direct download (10 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   114 citations  
  • Functional architectures for cognition: are simple inferences possible?Steven W. Zucker - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):153-154.
  • Abstract codes are not just for chimpanzees.Thomas R. Zentall - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):157-158.
  • The mystery-mastery-imagery complex.H. T. A. Whiting & R. P. Ingvaldsen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):228-229.
  • Cognition is not computation, for the reasons that computers don't solve the mind-body problems.Walter B. Weimer - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):152-153.
  • The pros and cons of having a word for it.S. F. Walker - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):156-157.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Potential disparities between imagining and preparing motor skills.Charles B. Walter & Stephan P. Swinnen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):227-228.
  • Eidetic imagery need not haunt us: a supportive example for the use of phenomenological reports.Benjamin Wallace - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):618-619.
  • Cognitive maps: dimensionality and development.J. Jacques Vonèche - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):519-520.
  • Imagery needs preparation too.Stefan Vogt - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):226-227.
  • Is hippocampal rhythmical slow activity specifically related to movement through space?C. M. Vanderwolf - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):518-519.
  • Action and attention.A. H. C. Van der Heijden & Bruce Bridgeman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):225-226.
  • Anatomical units in psychology.Holger Ursin - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):518-518.
  • The education of behaviorism and the nature of learning.William Timberlake - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):638-639.
  • “Model systems” versus “neuroethological” approach to hippocampal function.Richard F. Thompson, Paul R. Solomon & Donald J. Weisz - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):517-518.
  • Cognition, memory, and the hippocampus.Garth J. Thomas - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):515-517.
  • The anatomy of a cognitive map.L. W. Swanson - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):515-515.
  • Separability of reference frame distinctions from motor and visual images.Gary W. Strong - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):224-225.
  • Computation without representation.Stephen P. Stich - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):152-152.
  • The imagery debate.Kim Sterelny - 1986 - Philosophy of Science 53 (December):560-83.
    One central debate in cognitive science is over imagery. Do images constitute, or constitute evidence for, a distinctive, depictive form of mental representation? The most sophisticated advocacy of this view has been developed by Kosslyn and his coworkers. This paper focuses on his position and argues (i) that though Kosslyn has not developed a satisfactory account of depiction, there is nothing in principle unintelligible about the idea of depictive neural representation, but (ii) Kosslyn's model of imagery rescues the intelligibility of (...)
    Direct download (12 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Mental representation: What language is brainese?Kim Sterelny - 1983 - Philosophical Studies 43 (May):365-82.
  • A Cognitive Theory of Graphical and Linguistic Reasoning: Logic and Implementation.Keith Stenning & Jon Oberlander - 1995 - Cognitive Science 19 (1):97-140.
    We discuss external and internal graphical and linguistic representational systems. We argue that a cognitive theory of peoples' reasoning performance must account for (a) the logical equivalence of inferences expressed in graphical and linguistic form, and (b) the implementational differences that affect facility of inference. Our theory proposes that graphical representation limit abstraction and thereby aid “processibility”. We discuss the ideas of specificity and abstraction, and their cognitive relevance. Empirical support both comes from tasks which involve the manipulation of external (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   51 citations  
  • The hippocampus, space, and human amnesia.Larry R. Squire - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):514-515.
  • Computation and symbolization.William E. Smythe - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):151-152.
  • The need for strict differentiation between eidetics and noneidetics.Gudmund Smith - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):617-618.
  • A new abstract code or the new possibility of multiple codes?Annette Karmiloff Smith - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):149-150.
  • The tripartite model of representation.Peter Slezak - 2002 - Philosophical Psychology 15 (3):239-270.
    Robert Cummins [(1996) Representations, targets and attitudes, Cambridge, MA: Bradford/MIT, p. 1] has characterized the vexed problem of mental representation as "the topic in the philosophy of mind for some time now." This remark is something of an understatement. The same topic was central to the famous controversy between Nicolas Malebranche and Antoine Arnauld in the 17th century and remained central to the entire philosophical tradition of "ideas" in the writings of Locke, Berkeley, Hume, Reid and Kant. However, the scholarly, (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • How do we satisfy our goals?Paul G. Skokowski - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):224-224.
  • Canonical representations and constructive praxis: Some developmental and linguistic considerations.Chris Sinha - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):223-224.
  • The search for neurological correlates of eidetic imagery.Elsa M. Siipola - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):617-617.
  • Eidetic imagery: where's the ghost?Michael H. Siegel - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):616-617.
  • Eidetic imagery: continuing to be an enigmatic phenomenon.Peter W. Sheehan - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):615-616.
  • Neurophysiology of preparation, movement and imagery.Jerome N. Sanes - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):221-223.
  • Visual perspective-taking and image-like representations: We don't see it.Steven Samuel, Klara Hagspiel, Madeline J. Eacott & Geoff G. Cole - 2021 - Cognition 210 (C):104607.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Eidetic imagery is not a ghost.Paul A. Roodin & Erol F. Giray - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):614-615.
  • Kinaesthetic illusions as tools in understanding motor imagery.J. P. Roll, J. C. Gilhodes & R. Roll - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):220-221.
  • Does language training affect the code used by chimpanzees?: Some cautions and reservations.H. L. Roitblat - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):155-156.
  • Gallistel's metatheory of action.H. L. Roitblat - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):637-638.
  • The visualization continuum.Cynthia Roberts-Gray - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):614-614.
  • Doubts about the importance of language training and the abstract code.William A. Roberts - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):154-155.
  • Nonconscious motor images.Giacomo Rizzolatti - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):220-220.
  • Functional architecture and model validation.Martin Ringle - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):150-151.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Eidetic imagery, occipital EEG activity, and palinopsia.Alan Richardson - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):613-613.
  • Penetrating the impenetrable.Georges Rey - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):149-150.
  • Behavior ignored.Peter C. Reynolds - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):637-637.
  • To dream is not to (intend to) do.Jean Requin - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):218-219.
  • Can mental representations cause behavior?Edward S. Reed - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):635-636.
  • On O'Keefe, Nadel, space and brain.James B. Ranck - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):513-514.