Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Three key questions to move towards a theoretical framework of visuospatial perspective taking.Steven Samuel, Thorsten M. Erle, Louise P. Kirsch, Andrew Surtees, Ian Apperly, Henryk Bukowski, Malika Auvray, Caroline Catmur, Klaus Kessler & Francois Quesque - 2024 - Cognition 247 (C):105787.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Spontaneous perspective taking toward robots: The unique impact of humanlike appearance.Xuan Zhao & Bertram F. Malle - 2022 - Cognition 224 (C):105076.
  • Mentalizing Another's Visual World—A Novel Exploration via Motion Aftereffect.Xuefei Yuan, Nanbo Wang, Haiyan Geng & Shen Zhang - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Dissociation of posture remapping and cognitive load in level-2 perspective-taking.Yei-Yu Yeh, Chi-Chin Wang, Shih-Kuen Cheng & Chui-De Chiu - 2021 - Cognition 214 (C):104733.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Mental Rotation in False Belief Understanding.Jiushu Xie, Him Cheung, Manqiong Shen & Ruiming Wang - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (4):1179-1206.
    This study examines the spontaneous use of embodied egocentric transformation in understanding false beliefs in the minds of others. EET involves the participants mentally transforming or rotating themselves into the orientation of an agent when trying to adopt his or her visuospatial perspective. We argue that psychological perspective taking such as false belief reasoning may also involve EET because of what has been widely reported in the embodied cognition literature, showing that our processing of abstract, propositional information is often grounded (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Perspective taking as virtual navigation? Perceptual simulation of what others see reflects their location in space but not their gaze.Eleanor Ward, Giorgio Ganis, Katrina L. McDonough & Patric Bach - 2020 - Cognition 199 (C):104241.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Speakers' use of agency and visual context in spatial descriptions.Alessia Tosi, Martin J. Pickering & Holly P. Branigan - 2020 - Cognition 194 (C):104070.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Time pressure disrupts level-2, but not level-1, visual perspective calculation: A process-dissociation analysis.Andrew R. Todd, Austin J. Simpson & C. Daryl Cameron - 2019 - Cognition 189 (C):41-54.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Try to see it my way: Embodied perspective enhances self and friend-biases in perceptual matching.Yang Sun, Luis J. Fuentes, Glyn W. Humphreys & Jie Sui - 2016 - Cognition 153:108-117.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • An Externalist Theory of Social Understanding: Interaction, Psychological Models, and the Frame Problem.Axel Seemann - 2021 - Review of Philosophy and Psychology:1-25.
    I put forward an externalist theory of social understanding. On this view, psychological sense making takes place in environments that contain both agent and interpreter. The spatial structure of such environments is social, in the sense that its occupants locate its objects by an exercise in triangulation relative to each of their standpoints. This triangulation is achieved in intersubjective interaction and gives rise to a triadic model of the social mind. This model can then be used to make sense of (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • An Externalist Theory of Social Understanding: Interaction, Psychological Models, and the Frame Problem.Axel Seemann - 2023 - Review of Philosophy and Psychology 14 (1):139-163.
    I put forward an externalist theory of social understanding. On this view, psychological sense making takes place in environments that contain both agent and interpreter. The spatial structure of such environments is social, in the sense that its occupants locate its objects by an exercise in triangulation relative to each of their standpoints. This triangulation is achieved in intersubjective interaction and gives rise to a triadic model of the social mind. This model can then be used to make sense of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Visual perspective-taking and image-like representations: We don't see it.Steven Samuel, Klara Hagspiel, Madeline J. Eacott & Geoff G. Cole - 2021 - Cognition 210 (C):104607.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Listeners use speaker identity to access representations of spatial perspective during online language comprehension.Rachel A. Ryskin, Ranxiao Frances Wang & Sarah Brown-Schmidt - 2016 - Cognition 147 (C):75-84.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • TEST: A Tropic, Embodied, and Situated Theory of Cognition.Andriy Myachykov, Christoph Scheepers, Martin H. Fischer & Klaus Kessler - 2014 - Topics in Cognitive Science 6 (3):442-460.
    TEST is a novel taxonomy of knowledge representations based on three distinct hierarchically organized representational features: Tropism, Embodiment, and Situatedness. Tropic representational features reflect constraints of the physical world on the agent's ability to form, reactivate, and enrich embodied (i.e., resulting from the agent's bodily constraints) conceptual representations embedded in situated contexts. The proposed hierarchy entails that representations can, in principle, have tropic features without necessarily having situated and/or embodied features. On the other hand, representations that are situated and/or embodied (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Object’s symmetry alters spatial perspective-taking processes.Hiroyuki Muto, Soyogu Matsushita & Kazunori Morikawa - 2019 - Cognition 191 (C):103987.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The primacy of social over visual perspective-taking.Henrike Moll & Derya Kadipasaoglu - 2013 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7.
  • Shared contributions of the head and torso to spatial reference frames across spatial judgments.Matthew R. Longo, Sampath S. Rajapakse, Adrian J. T. Alsmith & Elisa R. Ferrè - 2020 - Cognition 204 (C):104349.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Hierarchical Integration of Communicative and Spatial Perspective‐Taking Demands in Sensorimotor Control of Referential Pointing.Rui(睿) Liu(刘), Sara Bögels, Geoffrey Bird, W. Pieter Medendorp & Ivan Toni - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (1):e13084.
    Recognized as a simple communicative behavior, referential pointing is cognitively complex because it invites a communicator to consider an addressee's knowledge. Although we know referential pointing is affected by addressees’ physical location, it remains unclear whether and how communicators’ inferences about addressees’ mental representation of the interaction space influence sensorimotor control of referential pointing. The communicative perspective-taking task requires a communicator to point at one out of multiple referents either to instruct an addressee which one should be selected (communicative, COM) (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Are implicit affective evaluations related to mental rotation performance?Leonardo Jost & Petra Jansen - 2021 - Consciousness and Cognition 94 (C):103178.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Dissociating visual perspective taking and belief reasoning using a novel integrated paradigm: A preregistered online study.Rachel Green, Daniel Joel Shaw & Klaus Kessler - 2023 - Cognition 235 (C):105397.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Social scaling of extrapersonal space: Target objects are judged as closer when the reference frame is a human agent with available movement potentialities.C. Fini, M. Brass & G. Committeri - 2015 - Cognition 134:50-56.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Level 2 perspective-taking distinguishes automatic and non-automatic belief-tracking.Katheryn Edwards & Jason Low - 2019 - Cognition 193 (C):104017.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Relating spatial perspective taking to the perception of other's affordances: providing a foundation for predicting the future behavior of others.Sarah H. Creem-Regehr, Kyle T. Gagnon, Michael N. Geuss & Jeanine K. Stefanucci - 2013 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7.
  • Spatial Perspective-Taking in Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders: The Predictive Role of Visuospatial and Motor Abilities.Ramona Cardillo, Cristiana Erbì & Irene C. Mammarella - 2020 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 14.
  • Fractionating the unitary notion of dissociation: disembodied but not embodied dissociative experiences are associated with exocentric perspective-taking.Jason J. Braithwaite, Kelly James, Hayley Dewe, Nick Medford, Chie Takahashi & Klaus Kessler - 2013 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7.
  • Perspective taking in language: integrating the spatial and action domains.Madeleine E. L. Beveridge & Martin J. Pickering - 2013 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7.
  • Dissociating contributions of head and torso to spatial reference frames: The misalignment paradigm.Adrian J. T. Alsmith, Elisa R. Ferrè & Matthew R. Longo - 2017 - Consciousness and Cognition 53:105-114.