Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Emotions before actions: When children see costs as causal.Claudia G. Sehl, Ori Friedman & Stephanie Denison - 2024 - Cognition 247 (C):105774.
  • Deciding to be authentic: Intuition is favored over deliberation when authenticity matters.Kerem Oktar & Tania Lombrozo - 2022 - Cognition 223 (C):105021.
    Deliberative analysis enables us to weigh features, simulate futures, and arrive at good, tractable decisions. So why do we so often eschew deliberation, and instead rely on more intuitive, gut responses? We propose that intuition might be prescribed for some decisions because people’s folk theory of decision-making accords a special role to authenticity, which is associated with intuitive choice. Five pre-registered experiments find evidence in favor of this claim. In Experiment 1 (N = 654), we show that participants prescribe intuition (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Interpersonal comparisons of well-being: Increasing convergence.Jelle de Boer - forthcoming - Politics, Philosophy and Economics.
    The main question of this paper is how people may agree in their interpersonal comparisons of wellbeing. These comparisons are important in social ethics and for policy purposes. The paper firstly examines grounds for convergence in easy cases. Then comes a more difficult case of low convergence in order to explore a way to increase it. For this, concepts from the empirical subjective well-being literature are used: life satisfaction and vignettes. Ideas of John Harsanyi and Serge Kolm thereby receive a (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Modeling Morality in 3‐D: Decision‐Making, Judgment, and Inference.Hongbo Yu, Jenifer Z. Siegel & Molly J. Crockett - 2019 - Topics in Cognitive Science 11 (2):409-432.
    The authors explore the interfaces between different dimensions of moral cognition, bridging economic, Bayesian and reinforcement learning perspectives. The human aversion to harming others cuts across these different interfaces, influencing decisions, judgments, and inferences about morality.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Outcome-Based Evaluations of Social Interaction Valence in a Contingent Response Context.Jun Yin, Xiaoyan He, Yisong Yang & Xiaoying Wu - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Commonsense psychology in human infants and machines.Gala Stojnić, Kanishk Gandhi, Shannon Yasuda, Brenden M. Lake & Moira R. Dillon - 2023 - Cognition 235 (C):105406.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Growing out of your own mind: Reexamining the development of the self-other difference in the unexpected contents task.David M. Sobel - 2023 - Cognition 235 (C):105403.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Inferences about moral character moderate the impact of consequences on blame and praise.Jenifer Z. Siegel, Molly J. Crockett & Raymond J. Dolan - 2017 - Cognition 167 (C):201-211.
    No categories
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Blind to Bias? Young Children Do Not Anticipate that Sunk Costs Lead to Irrational Choices.Claudia G. Sehl, Ori Friedman & Stephanie Denison - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (11):e13063.
    Cognitive Science, Volume 45, Issue 11, November 2021.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Pragmatic markers: the missing link between language and Theory of Mind.Paula Rubio-Fernandez - 2020 - Synthese 199 (1-2):1125-1158.
    Language and Theory of Mind come together in communication, but their relationship has been intensely contested. I hypothesize that pragmatic markers connect language and Theory of Mind and enable their co-development and co-evolution through a positive feedback loop, whereby the development of one skill boosts the development of the other. I propose to test this hypothesis by investigating two types of pragmatic markers: demonstratives and articles. Pragmatic markers are closed-class words that encode non-representational information that is unavailable to consciousness, but (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Thinking takes time: Children use agents' response times to infer the source, quality, and complexity of their knowledge.Emory Richardson & Frank C. Keil - 2022 - Cognition 224 (C):105073.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Not just if, but how much: Children and adults use cost and need to make evaluations about generosity across contexts.Mia Radovanovic, Tim Wei-Ting Chao, Emily Onyshko, Quang David Tri Huynh, Yang Leona Liu & Jessica A. Sommerville - 2023 - Cognition 238 (C):105533.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rational inferences about social valuation.Tadeg Quillien, John Tooby & Leda Cosmides - 2023 - Cognition 239 (C):105566.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rational information search in welfare-tradeoff cognition.Tadeg Quillien - 2023 - Cognition 231 (C):105317.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A simple definition of ‘intentionally’.Tadeg Quillien & Tamsin C. German - 2021 - Cognition 214 (C):104806.
    Cognitive scientists have been debating how the folk concept of intentional action works. We suggest a simple account: people consider that an agent did X intentionally to the extent that X was causally dependent on how much the agent wanted X to happen (or not to happen). Combined with recent models of human causal cognition, this definition provides a good account of the way people use the concept of intentional action, and offers natural explanations for puzzling phenomena such as the (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Interpersonal utility and children's social inferences from shared preferences.Madison L. Pesowski, Lindsey J. Powell, Mina Cikara & Adena Schachner - 2023 - Cognition 232 (C):105344.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Human-like machines: Transparency and comprehensibility.Piotr M. Patrzyk, Daniela Link & Julian N. Marewski - 2017 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 40.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rational learners and parochial norms.Scott Partington, Shaun Nichols & Tamar Kushnir - 2023 - Cognition 233 (C):105366.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neither neural networks nor the language-of-thought alone make a complete game.Iris Oved, Nikhil Krishnaswamy, James Pustejovsky & Joshua K. Hartshorne - 2023 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 46:e285.
    Cognitive science has evolved since early disputes between radical empiricism and radical nativism. The authors are reacting to the revival of radical empiricism spurred by recent successes in deep neural network (NN) models. We agree that language-like mental representations (language-of-thoughts [LoTs]) are part of the best game in town, but they cannot be understood independent of the other players.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Computational Models of Emotion Inference in Theory of Mind: A Review and Roadmap.Desmond C. Ong, Jamil Zaki & Noah D. Goodman - 2019 - Topics in Cognitive Science 11 (2):338-357.
    An important, but relatively neglected, aspect of human theory of mind is emotion inference: understanding how and why a person feels a certain why is central to reasoning about their beliefs, desires and plans. The authors review recent work that has begun to unveil the structure and determinants of emotion inference, organizing them within a unified probabilistic framework.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Moral alchemy: How love changes norms.Rachel W. Magid & Laura E. Schulz - 2017 - Cognition 167:135-150.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • People can use the placement of objects to infer communicative goals.Michael Lopez-Brau & Julian Jara-Ettinger - 2023 - Cognition 239 (C):105524.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Six-month-old infants expect agents to minimize the cost of their actions.Shari Liu & Elizabeth S. Spelke - 2017 - Cognition 160 (C):35-42.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Plans or Outcomes: How Do We Attribute Intelligence to Others?Marta Kryven, Tomer D. Ullman, William Cowan & Joshua B. Tenenbaum - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (9):e13041.
    Humans routinely make inferences about both the contents and the workings of other minds based on observed actions. People consider what others want or know, but also how intelligent, rational, or attentive they might be. Here, we introduce a new methodology for quantitatively studying the mechanisms people use to attribute intelligence to others based on their behavior. We focus on two key judgments previously proposed in the literature: judgments based on observed outcomes (you're smart if you won the game) and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Learning a commonsense moral theory.Max Kleiman-Weiner, Rebecca Saxe & Joshua B. Tenenbaum - 2017 - Cognition 167 (C):107-123.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Conviction Narrative Theory: A theory of choice under radical uncertainty.Samuel G. B. Johnson, Avri Bilovich & David Tuckett - 2023 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 46:e82.
    Conviction Narrative Theory (CNT) is a theory of choice underradical uncertainty– situations where outcomes cannot be enumerated and probabilities cannot be assigned. Whereas most theories of choice assume that people rely on (potentially biased) probabilistic judgments, such theories cannot account for adaptive decision-making when probabilities cannot be assigned. CNT proposes that people usenarratives– structured representations of causal, temporal, analogical, and valence relationships – rather than probabilities, as the currency of thought that unifies our sense-making and decision-making faculties. According to CNT, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • People learn other people’s preferences through inverse decision-making.Alan Jern, Christopher G. Lucas & Charles Kemp - 2017 - Cognition 168 (C):46-64.
    No categories
    Direct download (10 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • A computational framework for understanding the roles of simplicity and rational support in people's behavior explanations.Alan Jern, Austin Derrow-Pinion & A. J. Piergiovanni - 2021 - Cognition 210 (C):104606.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Sensitivity to the Sampling Process Emerges From the Principle of Efficiency.Julian Jara-Ettinger, Felix Sun, Laura Schulz & Joshua B. Tenenbaum - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (S1):270-286.
    Humans can seamlessly infer other people's preferences, based on what they do. Broadly, two types of accounts have been proposed to explain different aspects of this ability. The first account focuses on spatial information: Agents' efficient navigation in space reveals what they like. The second account focuses on statistical information: Uncommon choices reveal stronger preferences. Together, these two lines of research suggest that we have two distinct capacities for inferring preferences. Here we propose that this is not the case, and (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Not just what you did, but how: Children see distributors that count as more fair than distributors who don't.Colin Jacobs, Madison Flowers, Rosie Aboody, Maria Maier & Julian Jara-Ettinger - 2022 - Cognition 225 (C):105128.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The role of communication in acquisition, curation, and transmission of culture.Hyowon Gweon - 2020 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 43.
    Veissière et al.'s proposal aims to explain how cognition enables cultural learning, but fails to acknowledge a distinctively human behavior critical to this process: communication. Recent advances in developmental and computational cognitive science suggest that the social-cognitive capacities central to TTOM also support sophisticated yet remarkably early-emerging inferences and communicative behaviors that allow us to learn and share abstract knowledge.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What Could Go Wrong: Adults and Children Calibrate Predictions and Explanations of Others' Actions Based on Relative Reward and Danger.Nensi N. Gjata, Tomer D. Ullman, Elizabeth S. Spelke & Shari Liu - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (7):e13163.
    Cognitive Science, Volume 46, Issue 7, July 2022.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Learning the Structure of Social Influence.Samuel J. Gershman, Hillard Thomas Pouncy & Hyowon Gweon - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (S3):545-575.
    We routinely observe others’ choices and use them to guide our own. Whose choices influence us more, and why? Prior work has focused on the effect of perceived similarity between two individuals, such as the degree of overlap in past choices or explicitly recognizable group affiliations. In the real world, however, any dyadic relationship is part of a more complex social structure involving multiple social groups that are not directly observable. Here we suggest that human learners go beyond dyadic similarities (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • A rational model of people’s inferences about others’ preferences based on response times.Vael Gates, Frederick Callaway, Mark K. Ho & Thomas L. Griffiths - 2021 - Cognition 217 (C):104885.
  • Naïve information aggregation in human social learning.J. -Philipp Fränken, Simon Valentin, Christopher G. Lucas & Neil R. Bramley - 2024 - Cognition 242 (C):105633.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Action Generalization Across Group Members: Action Efficiency Matters.Jipeng Duan, Yingdong Jiang, Yunfeng He, Feng Zhang, Mowei Shen & Jun Yin - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (4):e12957.
    Actions are usually generalized among social group members. Importantly, the efficiency of an action with respect to achieving an external target determines action understanding, and it may have different degrees of social relevance to social groups. Thus, this study explored the role of action efficiency in action generalization. We used computer animations to simulate actions in social groups initiated by visual action cues or category labels, and we measured differences in response times between identifying actions that were and were not (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Identifying social partners through indirect prosociality: A computational account.Isaac Davis, Ryan Carlson, Yarrow Dunham & Julian Jara-Ettinger - 2023 - Cognition 240 (C):105580.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Intention beyond desire: Spontaneous intentional commitment regulates conflicting desires.Shaozhe Cheng, Minglu Zhao, Ning Tang, Yang Zhao, Jifan Zhou, Mowei Shen & Tao Gao - 2023 - Cognition 238 (C):105513.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Means-Inference as a Source of Variability in Early Helping.Sophie Bridgers & Hyowon Gweon - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The effects of information utility and teachers’ knowledge on evaluations of under-informative pedagogy across development.Ilona Bass, Elizabeth Bonawitz, Daniel Hawthorne-Madell, Wai Keen Vong, Noah D. Goodman & Hyowon Gweon - 2022 - Cognition 222 (C):104999.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A planning theory of belief.Sara Aronowitz - 2023 - Philosophical Perspectives 37 (1):5-17.
    What does it mean to hold a belief? Some of our ways of speaking in English suggest that to hold a belief is to have something in your mind: beliefs are things we acquire, defend, recover, and so on (Abelson, 1986). That is, believing is a matter of being in a state of having a thing. In this paper, I will argue for an alternative: believing is something we do. This is not a new suggestion. For instance, Matthew Boyle (2011) (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • How is mindreading really like reading?Ian A. Apperly - 2019 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 42.
    I suggest an alternative basis for Heyes’ analogy between cultural learning of mindreading and text reading. Unlike text reading, mindreading does not entail decoding of observable stimuli. Like text reading, mindreading requires relevant inferences. Identification of relevant inferences is a deeply challenging problem, and the most important contribution of cultural learning to mindreading may be an apprenticeship in thinking like a mindreader.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Having less means wanting more: Children hold an intuitive economic theory of diminishing marginal utility.Richard E. Ahl, Emma Cook & Katherine McAuliffe - 2023 - Cognition 234 (C):105367.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Learning about others: Modeling social inference through ambiguity resolution.Asya Achimova, Gregory Scontras, Christian Stegemann-Philipps, Johannes Lohmann & Martin V. Butz - 2022 - Cognition 218 (C):104862.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Preschoolers decide who is knowledgeable, who to inform, and who to trust via a causal understanding of how knowledge relates to action.Rosie Aboody, Holly Huey & Julian Jara-Ettinger - 2022 - Cognition 228 (C):105212.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Inference in the age of big data: Future perspectives on neuroscience.Danilo Bzdok & B. Yeo - unknown