Switch to: References

Citations of:

Psychology

Duke University Press (1892)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Synchronization and cognitive carpentry: From systematic structuring to simple reasoning. E. Koerner - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):465-466.
  • Thinking About Contradictions: The Imaginary Logic of Nikolai Aleksandrovich Vasil’Ev.Venanzio Raspa - 2017 - Cham, Switzerland: Springer Verlag.
    This volume examines the entire logical and philosophical production of Nikolai A. Vasil’ev, studying his life and activities as a historian and man of letters. Readers will gain a comprehensive understanding of this influential Russian logician, philosopher, psychologist, and poet. The author frames Vasil’ev’s work within its historical and cultural context. He takes into consideration both the situation of logic in Russia and the state of logic in Western Europe, from the end of the 19th century to the beginning of (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Ethereal oscillations.Malcolm P. Young - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):476-477.
  • Dynamic-binding theory is not plausible without chaotic oscillation.Ichiro Tsuda - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):475-476.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Should first-order logic be neurally plausible?David S. Touretzky & Scott E. Fahlman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):474-475.
  • Temporal synchrony and the speed of visual processing.Simon J. Thorpe - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):473-474.
  • Phase logic is biologically relevant logic.Gary W. Strong - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):472-473.
  • Do simple associations lead to systematic reasoning?Steven Sloman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):471-472.
  • From simple associations to systematic reasoning: A connectionist representation of rules, variables, and dynamic binding using temporal synchrony.Lokendra Shastri & Venkat Ajjanagadde - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):417-51.
    Human agents draw a variety of inferences effortlessly, spontaneously, and with remarkable efficiency – as though these inferences were a reflexive response of their cognitive apparatus. Furthermore, these inferences are drawn with reference to a large body of background knowledge. This remarkable human ability seems paradoxical given the complexity of reasoning reported by researchers in artificial intelligence. It also poses a challenge for cognitive science and computational neuroscience: How can a system of simple and slow neuronlike elements represent a large (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   121 citations  
  • A step toward modeling reflexive reasoning.Lokendra Shastri & Venkat Ajjanagadde - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):477-494.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The sense of society.Lloyd E. Sandelands - 1994 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 24 (4):305–338.
    Human society is unique in the animal kingdom in the degree to which it depends upon its members reflective awareness of self and society. Whereas much has been learned about the sense of self, little is known about the sense of society. This paper develops three points about the human sense of society: First, this sense is a feeling of life, what German writers have called Lebensgefuhl. The paper begins by defining feeling as a psychical moment or‘phase’of bodily activity. The (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Useful ideas for exploiting time to engineer representations.Richard Rohwer - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):471-471.
  • The Emergence of Words: Attentional Learning in Form and Meaning.Terry Regier - 2005 - Cognitive Science 29 (6):819-865.
    Children improve at word learning during the 2nd year of life—sometimes dramatically. This fact has suggested a change in mechanism, from associative learning to a more referential form of learning. This article presents an associative exemplar-based model that accounts for the improvement without a change in mechanism. It provides a unified account of children's growing abilities to (a) learn a new word given only 1 or a few training trials (“fast mapping”); (b) acquire words that differ only slightly in phonological (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • Making reasoning more reasonable: Event-coherence and assemblies.Günther Palm - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):470-470.
  • Psychological implications of the synchronicity hypothesis.Stellan Ohlsson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):469-469.
  • Computational and biological constraints in the psychology of reasoning.Mike Oaksford & Mike Malloch - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):468-469.
  • Sympathy, empathy, and the stream of consciousness.Thomas Natsoulas - 1988 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 18 (June):169-195.
  • What we know and the LTKB.Stanley Munsat - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):466-467.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The concept of 'function' and functional analysis in sociology.Peter A. Munch - 1976 - Philosophy of the Social Sciences 6 (3):193-213.
  • Some aspects of perception in old nyāya.Pradyot Kumar Mondal - 1982 - Journal of Indian Philosophy 10 (4):357-376.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Reflections on reflexive reasoning.David L. Martin - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):466-466.
  • The Impossible project of Ivan Pavlov.David Joravsky - 1992 - Science in Context 5 (2):265-280.
    The ArgumentIn different contexts, beginning with different concerns, Pavlov, James, and Freud tried to achieve a neurophysiological explanation of mind, and suffered defeat. James and Freud acknowledged the defeat and attempted, in radically different ways, to construct an interim psychology, hoping that neural explanation would be achieved in the future. Pavlov came to the effort in his fifties, after decades of research that took for granted a sharp separation between neurophysiology and psychology. He changed his mind as he noticed the (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Construction of Colorimetry by Committee.Sean F. Johnston - 1996 - Science in Context 9 (4):387-420.
    The ArgumentThis paper explores the confrontation of physical and contextual factors involved in the emergence of the subject of color measurement, which stabilized in essentially its present form during the interwar period. The contentions surrounding the specialty had both a national and a disciplinary dimension. German dominance was curtailed by American and British contributions after World War I. Particularly in America, communities of physicists and psychologists had different commitments to divergent views of nature and human perception. They therefore had to (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Distributing structure over time.John E. Hummel & Keith J. Holyoak - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):464-464.
  • On the artificial intelligence paradox.Steffen Hölldobler - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):463-464.
  • Not all reflexive reasoning is deductive.Graeme Hirst & Dekai Wu - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):462-463.
  • Rule acquisition and variable binding: Two sides of the same coin.P. J. Hampson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):462-462.
  • Competing, or perhaps complementary, approaches to the dynamic-binding problem, with similar capacity limitations.Graeme S. Halford - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):461-462.
  • Self-organizing neural models of categorization, inference and synchrony.Stephen Grossberg - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):460-461.
  • Must we solve the binding problem in neural hardware?James W. Garson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):459-460.
  • Deconstruction of neural data yields biologically implausible periodic oscillations.Walter J. Freeman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):458-459.
  • Toward a unified behavioral and brain science.Jerome A. Feldman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):458-458.
  • Dynamic bindings by real neurons: Arguments from physiology, neural network models and information theory.Reinhard Eckhorn - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):457-458.
  • Connectionism and syntactic binding of concepts.Georg Dorffner - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):456-457.
  • Reasoning, learning and neuropsychological plausibility.Joachim Diederich - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):455-456.
  • Making a middling mousetrap.Michael R. W. Dawson & Istvan Berkeley - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):454-455.
  • From symbols to neurons: Are we there yet?Garrison W. Cottrell - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):454-454.
  • Could static binding suffice?Paul R. Cooper - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):453-454.
  • Fluency, Satisfaction, Truth: Reassessing James in Light of Some Contemporary Psychology.Daniel J. Brunson - 2016 - Contemporary Pragmatism 13 (1):29-47.
    A notable feature of classical American pragmatism is its close association with the birth of experimental psychology. In particular, William James’ work as a psychologist influenced, and was influenced by, his pragmatism. This paper seeks to support this reading of the relation between Jamesian psychology and pragmatism, particularly through his “Sentiment of Rationality” and the later contention that the true is the satisfactory. In addition, James’ insights are tested and expanded through reference to contemporary research on processing fluency, as well (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Reconsidering Scenic Beauty.Arnold Berleant - 2010 - Environmental Values 19 (3):335 - 350.
    Attempts to justify the objectivity and universality of aesthetic judgment have traditionally rested on unsupported assumptions or mere assertion. This paper offers a fresh consideration of the problem of judgments of taste. It suggests that the problem of securing universal agreement is false and therefore insoluble since it imposes an inappropriate logical criterion on the extent of agreement, which is irrevocably empirical. The variability of judgments of taste actually forms a subject ripe for inquiry by sociologists, psychologists, historians and anthropologists, (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Plausible inference and implicit representation.Malcolm I. Bauer - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):452-453.
  • Time phases, pointers, rules and embedding.John A. Barnden - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):451-452.
    This paper is a commentary on the target article by Lokendra Shastri & Venkat Ajjanagadde [S&A]: “From simple associations to systematic reasoning: A connectionist representation of rules, variables and dynamic bindings using temporal synchrony” in same issue of the journal, pp.417–451. -/- It puts S&A's temporal-synchrony binding method in a broader context, comments on notions of pointing and other ways of associating information - in both computers and connectionist systems - and mentions types of reasoning that are a challenge to (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neurotheology: The working brain and the work of theology.James B. Ashbrook - 1984 - Zygon 19 (3):331-350.
    Because the mind is the significance of the brain and God is the significance of the mind, the concept “mind” bridges how the brain works and traditional patterns of belief. The left mind, which utilizes rational vigilance and the imperative instructions of proclamation, names and analyzes the urgently right. The right mind, which discloses the relational responsiveness of numinous presence and natural symbolism, is immersed in and integrates the ultimately real. Together they provide a typology of mind‐states with which to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • The Decolonization of Psychology or the Science of the Soul.Samuel Bendeck Sotillos - 2021 - Spirituality Studies 7 (1):18-37.
    Since the inception of psychology as a distinct field of study in the modern West, it has been widely regarded as the only valid form of this discipline, supplanting all other accounts of the mind and human behavior. The modern West is unique in having produced the only psychology that consciously severed itself from metaphysics and spiritual principles. The momentous intellectual revolutions inaugurated by the Renaissance and the European Enlightenment further entrenched the prejudices of its purely secular and reductionist approach. (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Structure and transition: towards an accretivist theory of time.David Preston Taylor - unknown
    This dissertation is a defense of a particular theory of the metaphysics of time which I call "accretivism", but which is popularly known in a form usually called the "Growing Block Theory". The goal of a metaphysics of time is to incorporate the various aspects of our temporal experience into a single, comprehensive whole. To this end I delineate five aspects of our ordinary experience of time: 1) The Tensed Aspect, in virtue of which objects are presented to us as (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark