Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Recovering from negative events by boosting implicit positive affect.Markus Quirin, Regina C. Bode & Julius Kuhl - 2011 - Cognition and Emotion 25 (3):559-570.
    Upregulation of implicit positive affect (PA) can act as a mechanism to deal with negative affect. Two studies tracked temporal changes in positive and negative affect (NA) assessed by self-report and the Implicit Positive and Negative Affect Test (IPANAT; Quirin, Kazén, & Kuhl, 2009). Study 1 observed the predicted increases in implicit PA after exposure to a threat-related film clip, which correlated positively with the speed of recognising a happy face among an angry crowd. Study 2 replicated increases in implicit (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • The epigenesis of regional specificity.Ralph-Axel Müller - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):650-675.
    Chomskyian claims of a genetically hard-wired and cognitively autonomous “universal grammar” are being promoted by generative linguistics as facts about language to the present day. The related doctrine of an evolutionary discontinuity in language emergence, however, is based on misconceptions about the notions of homology and preadaptation. The obvious lack of equivalence between symbolic communicative capacities in existing nonhuman primates and human language does not preclude common roots. Normal and disordered language development is strongly influenced by the genome, but there (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Genes, specificity, and the lexical/functional distinction in language acquisition.Karin Stromswold - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):648-649.
    Contrary to Müller's claims, and in support of modular theories, genetic factors play a substantial and significant role in language. The finding that some children with specific language impairment (SLI) have nonlinguistic impairments may reflect improper diagnosis of SLI or impairments that are secondary to linguistic impairments. Thus, such findings do not argue against the modularity thesis. The lexical/functional distinction appears to be innate and specifically linguistic and could be instantiated in either symbolic or connectionist systems.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Evolutionary principles and the emergence of syntax.P. Thomas Schoenemann & William S.-Y. Wang - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):646-647.
    The belief that syntax is an innate, autonomous, species-specific module is highly questionable. Syntax demonstrates the mosaic nature of evolutionary change, in that it made use of (and led to the enhancement of) numerous preexisting neurocognitive features. It is best understood as an emergent characteristic of the explosion of semantic complexity that occurred during hominid evolution.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Biology of language: Principle predictions and evidence.Friedemann Pulvermüller, Bettina Mohr & Hubert Preissl - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):643-645.
    Müller's target article aims to summarize approaches to the question of how language elements (phonemes, morphemes, etc.) and rules are laid down in the brain. However, it suffers from being too vague about basic assumptions and empirical predictions of neurobiological models, and the empirical evidence available to test the models is not appropriately evaluated. (1) In a neuroscientific model of language, different cortical localizations of words can only be based on biological principles. These need to be made explicit. (2) Evidence (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How to grow a human.Michael C. Corballis - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):632-633.
    I enlarge on the theme that the brain mechanisms required for languageand other aspects of the human mind evolved through selective changes in the regulatory genes governing growth. Extension of the period of postnatal growth increases the role of the environment in structuring the brain, and spatiotemporal programming (heterochrony) ofgrowth might explain hierarchical representation, hemispheric specialization, and perhaps sex differences.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Innateness, autonomy, universality? Neurobiological approaches to language.Ralph-Axel Müller - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):611-631.
    The concepts of the innateness, universality, species-specificity, and autonomy of the human language capacity have had an extreme impact on the psycholinguistic debate for over thirty years. These concepts are evaluated from several neurobiological perspectives, with an emphasis on the emergence of language and its decay due to brain lesion and progressive brain disease.Evidence of perceptuomotor homologies and preadaptations for human language in nonhuman primates suggests a gradual emergence of language during hominid evolution. Regarding ontogeny, the innate component of language (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Classical conditioning and the placebo effect.Ian Wickram - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):160-161.
  • Classical conditioning: A manifestation of Bayesian neural learning.James Christopher Westland & Manfred Kochen - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):160-160.
  • Is human language just another neurobiological specialization?Stephen F. Walker - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):649-650.
    One can disagree with Müller that it is neurobiologically questionable to suppose that human language is innate, specialized, and species-specific, yet agree that the precise brain mechanisms controlling language in any individual will be influenced by epigenesis and genetic variability, and that the interplay between inherited and acquired aspects of linguistic capacity deserves to be investigated.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Emotion-based learning: insights from the Iowa Gambling Task.Oliver H. Turnbull, Caroline H. Bowman, Shanti Shanker & Julie L. Davies - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Classical conditioning beyond the reflex: An uneasy rebirth.Jaylan Sheila Turkkan - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):161-179.
  • Classical conditioning: The new hegemony.Jaylan Sheila Turkkan - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):121-137.
    Converging data from different disciplines are showing the role of classical conditioning processes in the elaboration of human and animal behavior to be larger than previously supposed. Restricted views of classically conditioned responses as merely secretory, reflexive, or emotional are giving way to a broader conception that includes problem-solving, and other rule-governed behavior thought to be the exclusive province of either operant conditiońing or cognitive psychology. These new views have been accompanied by changes in the way conditioning is conducted and (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   57 citations  
  • The conditioned response: More than a knee-jerk in the ontogeny of behavior.William P. Smotherman & Scott R. Robinson - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):159-160.
  • A polyglot perspective on dissociation.Neil Smith - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):648-648.
    Evidence is presented from a polyglot savant to suggest that double dissociations between linguistic and nonverbal abilities are more important than Müller's target article implies. It is also argued that the special nature of syntax makes its assimilation to other aspects of language or to nonhuman communication systems radically implausible.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Autonomy and its discontents.Chris Sinha - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):647-648.
    Müller's review of the neuroscientific evidence undermines nativist claims for autonomous syntax and the argument from the poverty of the stimulus. Generativists will appeal to data from language acquisition, but here too there is growing evidence against the nativist position. Epigenetic naturalism, the developmental alternative to nativism, can be extended to epigenetic socionaturalism, acknowledging the importance of sociocultural processes in language and cognitive development.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The evolution of multiple memory systems.David F. Sherry & Daniel L. Schacter - 1987 - Psychological Review 94 (4):439-454.
  • Classical conditioning and language: The old hegemony.Vincent J. Samar & Gerald P. Berent - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):158-159.
  • Running from William James' Bear: A Review of Preattentive Mechanisms and their Contributions to Emotional Experience. [REVIEW]Michael D. Robinson - 1998 - Cognition and Emotion 12 (5):667-696.
  • It's a far cry from speech to language.Maritza Rivera-Gaxiola & Annette Karmiloff-Smith - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):645-646.
    We agree with Müller's epigenetic view of evolution and ontogeny and applaud his multilevel perspective. With him, we stress the importance in ontogeny of progressive specialisation rather than prewired structures. However, we argue that he slips from “speech” to “language” and that, in seeking homologies, these two levels need to be kept separate in the analysis of evolution and ontogeny.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Classical conditioning: A parsimonious analysis?Anthony L. Riley - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):157-158.
  • The cognitive unconscious: An evolutionary perspective.Arthur S. Reber - 1992 - Consciousness and Cognition 1 (2):93-133.
    In recent decades it has become increasingly clear that a substantial amount of cognitive work goes on independent of consciousness. The research has been carried out largely under two rubrics, implicit learning and implicit memory. The former has been concerned primarily with the acquisition of knowledge independent of awareness and the latter with the manner in which memories not readily available to conscious recall or recognition play a role in behavior; collectively these operations comprise the essential functions of the cognitive (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   104 citations  
  • Neurobiology and linguistics are not yet unifiable.David Poeppel - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):642-643.
    Neurobiological models of language need a level of analysis that can account for the typical range of language phenomena. Because linguistically motivated models have been successful in explaining numerous language properties, it is premature to dismiss them as biologically irrelevant. Models attempting to unify neurobiology and linguistics need to be sensitive to both sources of evidence.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Mis-representations.J. Bruce Overmier - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):156-157.
  • Müller's conclusions and linguistic research.Frederick J. Newmeyer - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):641-642.
    Because Müiller fails to distinguish between two senses of the term “autonomy,” there is a danger that his results will be misinterpreted by both linguists and neuroscientists. Although he may very well have been successful in refuting one sense of autonomy, he may actually have helped to provide an explanation for the correctness of autonomy in its other sense.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cerebro-cerebellar learning loops and language skills.John W. Moore - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):156-156.
  • Classical conditioning: The new hyperbole.Ralph R. Miller - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):155-156.
  • Contiguity, contingency, adaptiveness, and controls.Glenda MacQueen, James MacRae & Shepard Siegel - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):154-155.
  • The dark side of hegemony.Charles Locurto - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):153-154.
  • Extending the “new hegemony” of classical conditioning.Dan Lloyd - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):152-153.
  • Neuroanatomical structures and segregated circuits.Philip Lieberman - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):641-641.
    Segregated neural circuits that effect particular domain-specific behaviors can be differentiated from neuroanatomical structures implicated in many different aspects of behavior. The basal ganglionic components of circuits regulating nonlinguistic motor behavior, speech, and syntax all function in a similar manner. Hence, it is unlikely that special properties and evolutionary mechanisms are associated with the neural bases of human language.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cognitive-Emotional Interactions in the Brain.Joseph E. Ledoux - 1989 - Cognition and Emotion 3 (4):267-289.
  • Classical conditioning beyond the laboratory.Hugh Lacey - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):152-152.
  • Pavlovian conditioning: Providing a bridge between cognition and biology.Marvin D. Krank - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):151-151.
  • Beyond respondent conditioning.Sibylle Klosterhalfen & Wolfgang Klosterhalfen - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):149-150.
  • A promising new strategy for studying conditioned Immunomodulation.Wolfgang Klosterhalfen - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):150-150.
  • Decisions and the evolution of memory: Multiple systems, multiple functions.Stanley B. Klein, Leda Cosmides, John Tooby & Sarah Chance - 2002 - Psychological Review 109 (2):306-329.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   52 citations  
  • The importance of classical conditioning.H. D. Kimmel - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):148-149.
  • Complexity at the organismic and neuronal levels.R. W. Kentridge - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):147-148.
  • Innateness, autonomy, universality, and the neurobiology of regular and irregular inflectional morphology.David Kemmerer - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):639-641.
    Müller's goal of bringing neuroscience to bear on controversies in linguistics is laudable. However, some of his specific proposals about innateness and autonomy are misguided. Recent studies on the neurobiology of regular and irregular inflectional morphology indicate that these two linguistic processes are subserved by anatomically and physiologically distinct neural subsystems, whose functional organization is likely to be under direct genetic control rather than assembled by strictly epigenetic factors.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Associative theory versus classical conditioning: Their proper relationship.E. James Kehoe - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):147-147.
  • What is classical conditioning?W. J. Jacobs - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):146-146.
  • Pluripotentiality, epigenesis, and language acquisition.Bob Jacobs & Lori Larsen - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):639-639.
    Müller provides a valuable synthesis of neurobiological evidence on the epigenetic development of neural structures involved in language acquisition. The pluripotentiality of developing neural tissue crucially constrains linguistic/cognitive theorizing about supposedly innate neural mechanisms and contributes significantly to our understanding of experience–dependent processes involved in language acquisition. Without this understanding, any proposed explanation of language acquisition is suspect.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Factors contributing to the magnitude of conditional fear following a 24-h retention interval: The incubation effect.W. J. Jacobs & James R. Blackburn - 1988 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 26 (2):145-148.
  • Preparatory response hypotheses: A muddle of causal and functional analyses.Karen L. Hollis - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):145-146.
  • A worthy enterprise injured by overinterpretation and misrepresentation.Marc D. Hauser & Jon Sakata - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):638-638.
    The synthetic position adopted by Müller is weakened by a large number of overinterpretations and misrepresentations, together with a caricatured view of innateness and modularity.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neurobiological approaches to language: Falsehoods and fallacies.Yosef Grodzinsky - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):637-637.
    The conclusion that language is not really innate or modular is based on several fallacies. I show that the target article confuses communicative skills with linguistic abilities, and that its discussion of brain/language relations is replete with factual errors. I also criticize its attempt to contrast biological and linguistic principles. Finally, I argue that no case is made for the “alternative” approach proposed here.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Classical conditioning: The role of interdisciplinary theory.Stephen Grossberg - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):144-145.
  • Speaking of language: Thoughts on associations.Susan Graham & Diane Poulin-Dubois - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):636-636.
    Müller attempts to downplay cases of dissociation between language and cognition as evidence against the modularity of language. We review cases of associations between verbal and nonverbal abilities as further evidence against the notion of language as an autonomous subsystem. We also point out a discrepancy between his proposal of homologies between nonhuman primates' communication and human language and recent proposals on the evolution of language.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Familial language impairment: The evidence.Myrna Gopnik - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):635-636.
    Müller argues that general cognitive skills and linguistic skills are not necessarily independent. However, cross-linguistic evidence from an inherited specific language disorder affecting productive rules suggests significant degrees of modularity, innateness, and universality of language. Confident claims about the overall nature of such a complex system still await more interdisciplinary research.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark