Switch to: References

Citations of:

Nonanalytic cognition: Memory, perception, and concept learning

In Gordon H. Bower (ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Academic Press. pp. 18--1 (1984)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The return of the reinforcement theorists.C. D. L. Wynne - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):156-156.
  • An ecological approach to cognitive (im)penetrability.Rob Withagen & Claire F. Michaels - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (3):399-400.
    We offer an ecological (Gibsonian) alternative to cognitive (im)penetrability. Whereas Pylyshyn explains cognitive (im)penetrability by focusing solely on computations carried out by the nervous system, according to the ecological approach the perceiver as a knowing agent influences the entire animal-environmental system: in the determination of what constitutes the environment (affordances), what constitutes information, what information is detected and, thus, what is perceived.
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A mathematical theory of reinforcement: An unexpected place to find support for analogical memory coding.Donald M. Wilkie & Lisa M. Saksida - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):155-156.
  • Fifty years on: The new “principles of behavior”?J. H. Wearden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):155-155.
  • How general is a general theory of reinforcement?Stephen F. Walker - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):154-155.
  • Towards a dynamic connectionist model of memory.Douglas Vickers & Michael D. Lee - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (1):40-41.
    Glenberg's account falls short in several respects. Besides requiring clearer explication of basic concepts, his account fails to recognize the autonomous nature of perception. His account of what is remembered, and its description, is too static. His strictures against connectionist modeling might be overcome by combining the notions of psychological space and principled learning in an embodied and situated network.
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Animal-centered models of reinforcement.William Timberlake - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):153-154.
  • Short-term memory in human operant conditioning.Frode Svartdal - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):152-153.
  • Context effects in word identification and episodic recognition: A single dissociation.Veronica Stumpfel & Kim Kirsner - 1986 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 24 (3):175-178.
  • The scale of nature: Fitted parameters and dimensional correctness.D. W. Stephens - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):150-152.
  • Practical effects of response specification.Richard L. Shull - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):150-150.
  • Awareness and reinforcement.Charles P. Shimp - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):149-150.
  • Decomposing intuitive components in a conceptual problem solving task☆.Rolf Reber, Marie-Antoinette Ruch-Monachon & Walter J. Perrig - 2007 - Consciousness and Cognition 16 (2):294-309.
    Research into intuitive problem solving has shown that objective closeness of participants’ hypotheses were closer to the accurate solution than their subjective ratings of closeness. After separating conceptually intuitive problem solving from the solutions of rational incremental tasks and of sudden insight tasks, we replicated this finding by using more precise measures in a conceptual problem-solving task. In a second study, we distinguished performance level, processing style, implicit knowledge and subjective feeling of closeness to the solution within the problem-solving task (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Memory and the integration of response sequences.Phil Reed - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):148-149.
  • Components of activation: Repetition and priming effects in lexical decision and recognition.Roger Ratcliff, William Hockley & Gail McKoon - 1985 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 114 (4):435-450.
  • From overt behavior to hypothetical behavior to memory: Inference in the wrong direction.Howard Rachlin - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):147-148.
  • Problems and pitfalls for Killeen's mathematical principles of reinforcement.Joseph J. Pear - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):146-147.
  • Extension to multiple schedules: Some surprising (and accurate) predictions.John A. Nevin - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):145-146.
  • Killeen's theory provides an answer – and a question.Mary Ann Metzger & Terje Sagvolden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):144-145.
  • Context-specific learning and control: The roles of awareness, task relevance, and relative salience.Matthew J. C. Crump, Joaquín M. M. Vaquero & Bruce Milliken - 2008 - Consciousness and Cognition 17 (1):22-36.
    The processes mediating dynamic and flexible responding to rapidly changing task-environments are not well understood. In the present research we employ a Stroop procedure to clarify the contribution of context-sensitive control processes to online performance. In prior work Stroop interference varied as a function of probe location context, with larger Stroop interference occurring for contexts associated with a high proportion of congruent items [Crump, M. J., Gong, Z., & Milliken, B. . The context-specific proportion congruent stroop effect: location as a (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Cognitive niches: An ecological model of strategy selection.Julian N. Marewski & Lael J. Schooler - 2011 - Psychological Review 118 (3):393-437.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  • Toward an instance theory of automatization.Gordon D. Logan - 1988 - Psychological Review 95 (4):492-527.
  • The CODE theory of visual attention: An integration of space-based and object-based attention.Gordon D. Logan - 1996 - Psychological Review 103 (4):603-649.
  • An instance theory of attention and memory.Gordon D. Logan - 2002 - Psychological Review 109 (2):376-400.
  • Memories and functional response units.Kennon A. Lattal & Josele Abreu-Rodrigues - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):143-144.
  • Integration and specificity of retrieval in a memory-based model of reinforcement.Marvin D. Krank - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):142-143.
  • The self-consistency model of subjective confidence.Asher Koriat - 2012 - Psychological Review 119 (1):80-113.
  • The feeling of knowing: Some metatheoretical implications for consciousness and control.Asher Koriat - 2000 - Consciousness and Cognition 9 (2):149-171.
    The study of the feeling of knowing may have implications for some of the metatheoretical issues concerning consciousness and control. Assuming a distinction between information-based and experience-based metacognitive judgments, it is argued that the sheer phenomenological experience of knowing (''noetic feeling'') occupies a unique role in mediating between implicit-automatic processes, on the one hand, and explicit-controlled processes, on the other. Rather than reflecting direct access to memory traces, noetic feelings are based on inferential heuristics that operate implicitly and unintentionally. Once (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   107 citations  
  • Confidence in one’s social beliefs: Implications for belief justification.Asher Koriat & Shiri Adiv - 2012 - Consciousness and Cognition 21 (4):1599-1616.
    Philosophers commonly define knowledge as justified true beliefs. A heated debate exists, however, about what makes a belief justified. In this article, we examine the question of belief justification from a psychological perspective, focusing on the subjective confidence in a belief that the person has just formed. Participants decided whether to accept or reject a proposition depicting a social belief, and indicated their confidence in their choice. The task was repeated six times, and choice latency was measured. The results were (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Rats, responses and reinforcers: Using a little psychology on our subjects.Peter R. Killeen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):157-172.
  • Mathematical principles of reinforcement.Peter R. Killeen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):105-135.
    Effective conditioning requires a correlation between the experimenter's definition of a response and an organism's, but an animal's perception of its behavior differs from ours. These experiments explore various definitions of the response, using the slopes of learning curves to infer which comes closest to the organism's definition. The resulting exponentially weighted moving average provides a model of memory that is used to ground a quantitative theory of reinforcement. The theory assumes that: incentives excite behavior and focus the excitement on (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Can people strategically control the encoding and retrieval of some morphologic and typographic details of words?Jerwen Jou & Hector M. Cortes - 2012 - Consciousness and Cognition 21 (3):1280-1297.
    This study investigated whether the encoding and retrieval of plurality information and letter-case information of words in recognition memory can be inhibited. Response-deadline experiments using single words have indicted a controlled processing mode, whereas studies using meaningful sentences have indicated an automatic mode of processing plurality information. Two similar opposing views have existed on the processing of letter-case information. The abstractionist view contends that we retain the abstract lexical information and discard the superficial perceptual case information. The proceduralist view holds (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A cross-linguistic study of early word meaning: universal ontology and linguistic influence.Mutsumi Imai & Dedre Gentner - 1997 - Cognition 62 (2):169-200.
  • "Schema abstraction" in a multiple-trace memory model.Douglas L. Hintzman - 1986 - Psychological Review 93 (4):411-428.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   209 citations  
  • Reinforcement without representation.Stephen José Hanson - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):141-142.
  • Has learning been shown to be attractor modification within reinforcement modelling?Robert A. M. Gregson - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):140-141.
  • Moving beyond schedules and rate: A new trajectory?Gregory Galbicka - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):139-140.
  • What do reinforcers strengthen? The unit of selection.John W. Donahoe - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):138-139.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What defines a legitimate issue for Skinnerian psychology: Philosophy or technology?Hank Davis - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):137-138.
  • Validation of behavioural equations: Can neurobiology help?C. M. Bradshaw - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):136-137.
  • Cognitive Strategies and Natural Environments Interact in Influencing Executive Function.Stefan C. Bourrier, Marc G. Berman & James T. Enns - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
  • The response problem.Robert C. Bolles - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):135-136.
  • Word recognition in early reading: A review of the direct and indirect access hypotheses. [REVIEW]Roderick W. Barron - 1986 - Cognition 24 (1-2):93-119.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Effects of attentive encoding on analytic and nonanalytic processing in implicit and explicit retrieval tasks.Linda J. Anooshian - 1989 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 27 (1):5-8.