Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. On mechanisms of cultural evolution, and the evolution of language and the common law.Michael T. Ghiselin - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):11-11.
  • Epigenesis and culture.Robert Fagen - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):10-10.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Thought Experiments in Biology.Guillaume Schlaepfer & Marcel Weber - 2018 - In Michael T. Stuart, Yiftach Fehige & James Robert Brown (eds.), The Routledge Companion to Thought Experiments. London: Routledge. pp. 243-256.
    Unlike in physics, the category of thought experiment is not very common in biology. At least there are no classic examples that are as important and as well-known as the most famous thought experiments in physics, such as Galileo’s, Maxwell’s or Einstein’s. The reasons for this are far from obvious; maybe it has to do with the fact that modern biology for the most part sees itself as a thoroughly empirical discipline that engages either in real natural history or in (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The place of mind, and the limits of amplification.Joachim F. Wohlwill - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):30-31.
  • The janus-face of philosophy of biology. [REVIEW]Patricia Williams - 1991 - Biology and Philosophy 6 (3):351-361.
  • Genes, mind, and culture; A turning point.Thomas Rhys Williams - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):29-30.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Resistance to biological self-understanding.Pierre L. van den Berghe - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):27-27.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Information, feedback, and transparency.Robert Van Gulick - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):27-29.
  • Darwin Online and the Evolution of the Darwin Industry.John van Wyhe - 2009 - History of Science 47 (4):459.
  • Mind and the linkage between genes and culture.John Maynard Smith - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):20-21.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Current Status of Research in Teaching and Learning Evolution: I. Philosophical/Epistemological Issues.Mike U. Smith - 2010 - Science & Education 19 (6-8):523-538.
  • A bully pulpit.L. B. Slobodkin - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):26-27.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Collaboration between biology and the social sciences: A milestone.Joseph Shepher - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):25-26.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The origin of theOrigin revisited.Silvan S. Schweber - 1977 - Journal of the History of Biology 10 (2):229-316.
  • Epigenesis: The newer synthesis?Glendon Schubert - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):24-25.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Poulett Scrope on the Volcanoes of Auvergne: Lyellian Time and Political Economy.Martin J. S. Rudwigk - 1974 - British Journal for the History of Science 7 (3):205-242.
    Early in 1826, at the age of 28, Charles Lyell began writing the first of a series of articles for J. G. Lockhart, the new editor of theQuarterly review. These articles gave him his first opportunity to express to the educated public his views on the state of science in general, and of geology in particular, in English society. According to the convention of theQuarterly, each article was nominally a review of one or more recently published works, but like other (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Are there culturgens?Alexander Rosenberg - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):22-24.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Journey from Discovery to Scientific Change: Scientific Communities, Shared Models, and Specialised Vocabulary.Sarah M. Roe - 2017 - International Studies in the Philosophy of Science 31 (1):47-67.
    Scientific communities as social groupings and the role that such communities play in scientific change and the production of scientific knowledge is currently under debate. I examine theory change as a complex social interaction among individual scientists and the scientific community, and argue that individuals will be motivated to adopt a more radical or innovative attitude when confronted with striking similarities between model systems and a more robust understanding of specialised vocabulary. Two case studies from the biological sciences, Barbara McClintock (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Genes, mind, and emotion.Robert Plutchik - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):21-22.
  • The Early History of Chance in Evolution.Charles H. Pence - 2015 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A 50:48-58.
    Work throughout the history and philosophy of biology frequently employs ‘chance’, ‘unpredictability’, ‘probability’, and many similar terms. One common way of understanding how these concepts were introduced in evolution focuses on two central issues: the first use of statistical methods in evolution (Galton), and the first use of the concept of “objective chance” in evolution (Wright). I argue that while this approach has merit, it fails to fully capture interesting philosophical reflections on the role of chance expounded by two of (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Sir John F. W. Herschel and Charles Darwin: Nineteenth-Century Science and Its Methodology.Charles H. Pence - 2018 - Hopos: The Journal of the International Society for the History of Philosophy of Science 8 (1):108-140.
    There are a bewildering variety of claims connecting Darwin to nineteenth-century philosophy of science—including to Herschel, Whewell, Lyell, German Romanticism, Comte, and others. I argue here that Herschel’s influence on Darwin is undeniable. The form of this influence, however, is often misunderstood. Darwin was not merely taking the concept of “analogy” from Herschel, nor was he combining such an analogy with a consilience as argued for by Whewell. On the contrary, Darwin’s Origin is written in precisely the manner that one (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The Scientific Self: Reclaiming Its Place in the History of Research Ethics.Herman Paul - 2018 - Science and Engineering Ethics 24 (5):1379-1392.
    How can the history of research ethics be expanded beyond the standard narrative of codification—a story that does not reach back beyond World War II—without becoming so broad as to lose all distinctiveness? This article proposes a history of research ethics focused on the “scientific self,” that is, the role-specific identity of scientists as typically described in terms of skills, competencies, qualities, or dispositions. Drawing on three agenda-setting texts from nineteenth-century history, biology, and sociology, the article argues that the “revolutions” (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The metaphor of the architect in Darwin: Chance and free will.Ricardo Noguera-Solano - 2013 - Zygon 48 (4):859-874.
    In The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication, published in 1868, Darwin used the metaphor of the architect to argue in favor of natural autonomy and to clarify the role of chance in his theory of adaptive change by variation and natural selection. In this article, I trace the history of this important heuristic instrument in Darwin's writings and letters and suggest that this metaphor was important to Darwin because it helps him to explain the role of chance, and (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Positive heuristics in evolutionary biology.Richard E. Michod - 1981 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 32 (1):1-36.
  • Toward a natural science of human culture.Roger D. Masters - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):19-20.
  • The power of reduction and the limits of compressibility.Hubert Markl - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):18-19.
  • Précis of Genes, Mind, and Culture.Charles J. Lumsden & Edward O. Wilson - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):1-7.
    Despite its importance, the linkage between genetic and cultural evolution has until now been little explored. An understanding of this linkage is needed to extend evolutionary theory so that it can deal for the first time with the phenomena of mind and human social history. We characterize the process of gene-culture coevolution, in which culture is shaped by biological imperatives while biological traits are simultaneously altered by genetic evolution in response to cultural history. A case is made from both theory (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   53 citations  
  • Genes and culture, protest and communication.Charles J. Lumsden & Edward O. Wilson - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):31-37.
    Despite its importance, the linkage between genetic and cultural evolution has until now been little explored. An understanding of this linkage is needed to extend evolutionary theory so that it can deal for the first time with the phenomena of mind and human social history. We characterize the process of gene-culture coevolution, in which culture is shaped by biological imperatives while biological traits are simultaneously altered by genetic evolution in response to cultural history. A case is made from both theory (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Top-down guidance from a bottom-up theory.Geoffrey R. Loftus - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):17-18.
  • From genes to culture: The missing links.Joseph K. Kovach - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):15-17.
  • The logic of discovery and Darwin's pre-malthusian researches.Scott A. Kleiner - 1988 - Biology and Philosophy 3 (3):293-315.
    Traditional logical empiricist and more recent historicist positions on the logic of discovery are briefly reviewed and both are found wanting. None have examined the historical detail now available from recent research on Darwin, from which there is evidence for gradual transition in descriptive and explanatory concepts. This episode also shows that revolutionary research can be directed by borrowed metascientific objectives and heuristics from other disciplines. Darwin's own revolutionary research took place within an ontological context borrowed from non evolutionary predecessors (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Goldschmidt’s Heresy and the Explanatory Promise of Ontogenetic Evolutionary Theory.Scott E. Kleiner - 1996 - Philosophica 58 (2).
  • Essay review - the philosophy of biology.Scott A. Kleiner - 1975 - Southern Journal of Philosophy 13 (4):523-542.
  • The Preface to Darwin’s Origin of Species: The Curious History of the “Historical Sketch”. [REVIEW]Curtis N. Johnson - 2007 - Journal of the History of Biology 40 (3):529 - 556.
    Almost any modern reader's first encounter with Darwin's writing is likely to be the "Historical Sketch," inserted by Darwin as a preface to an early edition of the Origin of Species, and having since then appeared as the preface to every edition after the second English edition. The Sketch was intended by him to serve as a short "history of opinion" on the species question before he presented his own theory in the Origin proper. But the provenance of the "Historical (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Concepts of development in the mathematics of cultural change.Timothy D. Johnston - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):14-15.
  • The Darwinian Revolution Revisited.Sandra Herbert - 2005 - Journal of the History of Biology 38 (1):51 - 66.
    The "Darwinian revolution" remains an acceptable phrase to describe the change in thought brought about by the theory of evolution, provided that the revolution is seen as occurring over an extended period of time. The decades from the 1790s through the 1850s are at the focus of this article. Emphasis is placed on the issue of species extinction and on generational shifts in opinion.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • A too simple view of population genetics.Daniel L. Hartl - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):13-14.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The “culturgen”: Science or science fiction?C. R. Hallpike - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):12-13.
  • Genes for general intellect rather than particular culture.Howard E. Gruber - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):11-12.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • David Hull’s Natural Philosophy of Science.Paul E. Griffiths - 2000 - Biology and Philosophy 15 (3):301-310.
    Throughout his career David Hull has sought to bring the philosophy of science into closer contact with science and especially with biological science (Hull 1969, 1997b). This effort has taken many forms. Sometimes it has meant ‘either explaining basic biology to philosophers or explaining basic philosophy to biologists’ (Hull 1996, p. 77). The first of these tasks, simple as it sounds, has been responsible for revolutionary changes. It is well known that traditional philosophy of science, modeled as it was on (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • The Darwinian tension.Hajo Greif - 2015 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 53:53-61.
    There have been attempts to subsume Charles Darwin's theory of evolution under either one of two distinct intellectual traditions: early Victorian natural science and its descendants in political economy (as exemplified by Herschel, Lyell, or Malthus) and the romantic approach to art and science emanating from Germany (as exemplified by Humboldt and Goethe). In this paper, it will be shown how these traditions may have jointly contributed to the design of Darwin's theory. The hypothesis is that their encounter created a (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • A Life More Ordinary: The Dull Life but Interesting Times of Joseph Dalton Hooker. [REVIEW]Jim Endersby - 2011 - Journal of the History of Biology 44 (4):611 - 631.
    The life of Joseph Dalton Hooker (1817-1911) provides an invaluable lens through which to view mid-Victorian science. A biographical approach makes it clear that some well-established narratives about this period need revising. For example, Hooker's career cannot be considered an example of the professionalisation of the sciences, given the doubtful respectability of being paid to do science and his reliance on unpaid collectors with pretensions to equal scientific and/or social status. Nor was Hooker's response to Darwin's theories either straightforward or (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A tale of two controversies: Dissonance in the theory and practice of rationality.Martin Eger - 1988 - Zygon 23 (3):291-325.
    The relation between rationality in science and rationality in moral discourse is of interest to philosophers and sociologists of science, to educators and moral philosophers. Apparently conflicting conceptions of rationality can be detected at the core of two current socio-educational controversies: the creationievolution controversy and that concerning “moral education.” This paper takes as its starting point the recorded views of participants in these controversies; exhibits the contradictions and their effect on the public; relates these contradictions to developments in the philosophy (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • En evolutionair geloof? Over 'intelligent design', darwinisme en theïsme.Andreas de Block - 2008 - Bijdragen 69 (1):3-17.
    Both the so-called high priests of atheism and the proponents of Intelligent Design argue that the Darwinian theory of evolution is more problematic for theism than any other scientific theory. Against the grain of most contemporary philosophers and theologians, I contend that their arguments are largely correct. Moreover, neo-Darwinism is especially threatening the soft theism or deism, defended by Darwin and several of the most prominent Darwinian theorists . For the proponents of ID, this implies that a more theistic science (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The epigenetic connection between genes and culture: Environment to the rescue.William R. Charlesworth - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):9-10.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Stalking the wild culturgen.Arthur L. Caplan - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):8-9.
  • Divided We Stand.Werner Callebaut - 2010 - Biological Theory 5 (4):295-295.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Beyond Generalized Darwinism. I. Evolutionary Economics from the Perspective of Naturalistic Philosophy of Biology.Werner Callebaut - 2011 - Biological Theory 6 (4):338-350.
    This is the first of two articles in which I reflect on “generalized Darwinism” as currently discussed in evolutionary economics. I approach evolutionary economics by the roundabouts of evolutionary epistemology and the philosophy of biology, and contrast evolutionary economists’ cautious generalizations of Darwinism with “imperialistic” proposals to unify the behavioral sciences. I then discuss the continued resistance to biological ideas in the social sciences, focusing on the issues of naturalism and teleology. In the companion article (Callebaut, Biol Theory 6. doi: (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Editorial introduction.Richard M. Burian & Joseph C. Pitt - 1992 - Synthese 92 (1):3-7.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Editorial introduction: Philosophy of Biology in Historical and Cultural Contexts.Richard M. Burian & Marjorie Grene - 1992 - Synthese 91 (1-2):1-7.