Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Synchronization and cognitive carpentry: From systematic structuring to simple reasoning. E. Koerner - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):465-466.
  • Cortical connections and parallel processing: Structure and function.Dana H. Ballard - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):67-90.
    The cerebral cortex is a rich and diverse structure that is the basis of intelligent behavior. One of the deepest mysteries of the function of cortex is that neural processing times are only about one hundred times as fast as the fastest response times for complex behavior. At the very least, this would seem to indicate that the cortex does massive amounts of parallel computation.This paper explores the hypothesis that an important part of the cortex can be modeled as a (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   106 citations  
  • What's in the term connectionist?.Christof Koch - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):100-101.
  • Value units make the right connections.Dana H. Ballard - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):107-120.
    The cerebral cortex is a rich and diverse structure that is the basis of intelligent behavior. One of the deepest mysteries of the function of cortex is that neural processing times are only about one hundred times as fast as the fastest response times for complex behavior. At the very least, this would seem to indicate that the cortex does massive amounts of parallel computation.This paper explores the hypothesis that an important part of the cortex can be modeled as a (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Putting together connectionism – again.Paul Smolensky - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):59-74.
    A set of hypotheses is formulated for a connectionist approach to cognitive modeling. These hypotheses are shown to be incompatible with the hypotheses underlying traditional cognitive models. The connectionist models considered are massively parallel numerical computational systems that are a kind of continuous dynamical system. The numerical variables in the system correspond semantically to fine-grained features below the level of the concepts consciously used to describe the task domain. The level of analysis is intermediate between those of symbolic cognitive models (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Does connectionism suffice?Steven W. Zucker - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):301-302.
  • Ethereal oscillations.Malcolm P. Young - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):476-477.
  • Sensory Analysis: A psychoacoustic view.William A. Yost - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (2):315-316.
  • The reality of the symbolic and subsymbolic systems.Andrew Woodfield & Adam Morton - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):58-58.
  • Is economics still immersed in the old concepts of the Enlightenment era?Andrzej P. Wierzbicki - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):236-237.
  • Visuomotor extrapolation.David Whitney - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (2):220-221.
    Accurate perception of moving objects would be useful; accurate visually guided action is crucial. Visual motion across the scene influences perceived object location and the trajectory of reaching movements to objects. In this commentary, I propose that the visual system assigns the position of any object based on the predominant motion present in the scene, and that this is used to guide reaching movements to compensate for delays in visuomotor processing.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Critical assumptions in psychophysical analysis.Peter Wenderoth - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (2):314-315.
  • What is Weber's Law?R. J. Watt - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (2):313-314.
  • Presupposing Weber's Law: Theory without independent confirmation is circular.Mark Wagner - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (2):312-313.
  • Flexible color perception depending on the shape and positioning of achromatic contours.Mark Vergeer, Stuart Anstis & Rob van Lier - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Has the case been made against the ecumenical view of connectionism?Robert Van Gulick - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):57-58.
  • A differentiated view of Weber's Law.Christopher W. Tyler - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (2):311-312.
  • Dynamic-binding theory is not plausible without chaotic oscillation.Ichiro Tsuda - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):475-476.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Connectionist computing and neural machinery: Examining the test of “timing”.John K. Tsotsos - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):106-107.
  • A contour propagation approach to surface filling-in and volume formation.Peter Ulric Tse - 2002 - Psychological Review 109 (1):91-115.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Should first-order logic be neurally plausible?David S. Touretzky & Scott E. Fahlman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):474-475.
  • On the proper treatment of thermostats.David S. Touretzky - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):55-56.
    A set of hypotheses is formulated for a connectionist approach to cognitive modeling. These hypotheses are shown to be incompatible with the hypotheses underlying traditional cognitive models. The connectionist models considered are massively parallel numerical computational systems that are a kind of continuous dynamical system. The numerical variables in the system correspond semantically to fine-grained features below the level of the concepts consciously used to describe the task domain. The level of analysis is intermediate between those of symbolic cognitive models (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sensory Analysis: The question of balance.David L. Tomko - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (2):311-311.
  • The human being as a bumbling optimalist: A psychologist's viewpoint.Masanao Toda - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):235-235.
  • Temporal synchrony and the speed of visual processing.Simon J. Thorpe - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):473-474.
  • What does the cortex do?Mriganka Sur - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):105-105.
  • Phase logic is biologically relevant logic.Gary W. Strong - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):472-473.
  • Optimal confusion.Stephanie Stolarz-Fantino & Edmund Fantino - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):234-234.
  • From data to dynamics: The use of multiple levels of analysis.Gregory O. Stone - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):54-55.
  • From connectionism to eliminativism.Stephen P. Stich - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):53-54.
  • Differential coupling for detection versus discrimination.Kent A. Stevens - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (2):310-311.
  • Avoid the push-pull dilemma in explanation.Kenneth M. Steele - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):233-234.
  • Extremum descriptions, process laws and minimality heuristics.Elliott Sober - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):232-233.
    The examples and concepts that Shoemaker cites are rather heterogeneous. Some distinctions need to be drawn. An optimality thesis involves not just an ordering of options, but a value judgment about them. So let us begin by distinguishing minimality from optimality. And the concept of minimality can play a variety of roles, among which I distinguish between extremum descriptions, statements hypothesizing an optimizing process, and methodological recommendations. Finally, I consider how the three categories relate to Shoemaker’s question that “Who is (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the proper treatment of connectionism.Paul Smolensky - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):1-23.
    A set of hypotheses is formulated for a connectionist approach to cognitive modeling. These hypotheses are shown to be incompatible with the hypotheses underlying traditional cognitive models. The connectionist models considered are massively parallel numerical computational systems that are a kind of continuous dynamical system. The numerical variables in the system correspond semantically to fine-grained features below the level of the concepts consciously used to describe the task domain. The level of analysis is intermediate between those of symbolic cognitive models (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   741 citations  
  • Do simple associations lead to systematic reasoning?Steven Sloman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):471-472.
  • How fully should connectionism be activated? Two sources of excitation and one of inhibition.Roger N. Shepard - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):52-52.
  • Rational agents, real people and the quest for optimality.Eldar Shafir - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):232-232.
  • From simple associations to systematic reasoning: A connectionist representation of rules, variables, and dynamic binding using temporal synchrony.Lokendra Shastri & Venkat Ajjanagadde - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):417-51.
    Human agents draw a variety of inferences effortlessly, spontaneously, and with remarkable efficiency – as though these inferences were a reflexive response of their cognitive apparatus. Furthermore, these inferences are drawn with reference to a large body of background knowledge. This remarkable human ability seems paradoxical given the complexity of reasoning reported by researchers in artificial intelligence. It also poses a challenge for cognitive science and computational neuroscience: How can a system of simple and slow neuronlike elements represent a large (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   121 citations  
  • A step toward modeling reflexive reasoning.Lokendra Shastri & Venkat Ajjanagadde - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):477-494.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Computational neuroscience.Terrence J. Sejnowski - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):104-105.
  • The strategy of optimality revisited.Paul J. H. Schoemaker - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):237-245.
  • The quest for optimality: A positive heuristic of science?Paul J. H. Schoemaker - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):205-215.
    This paper examines the strengths and weaknesses of one of science's most pervasive and flexible metaprinciples;optimalityis used to explain utility maximization in economics, least effort principles in physics, entropy in chemistry, and survival of the fittest in biology. Fermat's principle of least time involves both teleological and causal considerations, two distinct modes of explanation resting on poorly understood psychological primitives. The rationality heuristic in economics provides an example from social science of the potential biases arising from the extreme flexibility of (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • Structure and controlling subsymbolic processing.Walter Schneider - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):51-52.
  • Should the quest for optimality worry us?Nils-Eric Sahlin - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):231-231.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Making the connections.Jay G. Rueckl - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):50-51.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A cortical edge-integration model of object-based lightness computation that explains effects of spatial context and individual differences.Michael E. Rudd - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8.
  • Illusory Streaks from Corners and Their Perceptual Integration.Sergio Roncato, Stefano Guidi, Oronzo Parlangeli & Luca Battaglini - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Useful ideas for exploiting time to engineer representations.Richard Rohwer - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):471-471.
  • Optimality as a prescriptive tool.Alexander H. G. Rinnooy Kan - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):230-231.
  • On computer science, visual science, and the physiological utility of models.Barry J. Richmond & Michael E. Goldberg - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):300-301.