Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The meaning of representation in animal memory.H. L. Roitblat - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):353-372.
    A representation is a remnant of previous experience that allows that experience to affect later behavior. This paper develops a metatheoretical view of representation and applies it to issues concerning representation in animals. To describe a representational system one must specify the following: thedomainor range of situations in the represented world to which the system applies; thecontentor set of features encoded and preserved by the system; thecodeor transformational rules relating features of the representation to the corresponding features of the represented (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   312 citations  
  • Putting together connectionism – again.Paul Smolensky - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):59-74.
    A set of hypotheses is formulated for a connectionist approach to cognitive modeling. These hypotheses are shown to be incompatible with the hypotheses underlying traditional cognitive models. The connectionist models considered are massively parallel numerical computational systems that are a kind of continuous dynamical system. The numerical variables in the system correspond semantically to fine-grained features below the level of the concepts consciously used to describe the task domain. The level of analysis is intermediate between those of symbolic cognitive models (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • P3 and (de)activation.Walton T. Roth & Judith M. Ford - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):393.
  • Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating?Emanuel Donchin & Michael G. H. Coles - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):357.
    To understand the endogenous components of the event-related brain potential (ERP), we must use data about the components' antecedent conditions to form hypotheses about the information-processing function of the underlying brain activity. These hypotheses, in turn, generate testable predictions about the consequences of the component. We review the application of this approach to the analysis of the P300 component. The amplitude of the P300 is controlled multiplicatively by the subjective probability and the task relevance of the eliciting events, whereas its (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   193 citations  
  • Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating?Emanuel Donchin & Michael G. H. Coles - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):357.
    To understand the endogenous components of the event-related brain potential (ERP), we must use data about the components' antecedent conditions to form hypotheses about the information-processing function of the underlying brain activity. These hypotheses, in turn, generate testable predictions about the consequences of the component. We review the application of this approach to the analysis of the P300 component. The amplitude of the P300 is controlled multiplicatively by the subjective probability and the task relevance of the eliciting events, whereas its (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Event-related potentials and cognition: A critique of the context updating hypothesis and an alternative interpretation of P3.Rolf Verleger - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):343.
    P3 is the most prominent of the electrical potentials of the human electroencephalogram that are sensitive to psychological variables. According to the most influential current hypothesis about its psychological significance [E. Donchin's], the “context updating” hypothesis, P3 reflects the updating of working memory. This hypothesis cannot account for relevant portions of the available evidence and it entails some basic contradictions. A more general formulation of this hypothesis is that P3 reflects the updating of expectancies. This version implies that P3-evoking stimuli (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   115 citations  
  • What are the contributions of the direct perception approach?Carl B. Zuckerman - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):407-408.
  • The computational/representational paradigm as normal science: further support.Steven W. Zucker - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):406-407.
  • Functional architectures for cognition: are simple inferences possible?Steven W. Zucker - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):153-154.
  • Does connectionism suffice?Steven W. Zucker - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):301-302.
  • The heuristic value of representation.Thomas R. Zentall - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):393-394.
  • Attention to detail?Malcolm P. Young, Ian R. Paterson & David I. Perrett - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):417-418.
  • The nature of cerebral hemispheric specialisation in man: Quantitative vs. qualitative differences.Maria A. Wyke - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):78-79.
  • The reality of the symbolic and subsymbolic systems.Andrew Woodfield & Adam Morton - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):58-58.
  • Selective auditory attention: Complex processes and complex ERP generators.David L. Woods - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):260-261.
  • Attentional influence on the mismatch negativity.Marty G. Woldorff & Steven A. Hillyard - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):258-260.
  • Where's the psychological reality?C. Philip Winder - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):417-417.
  • Mind the brain.Martha Wilson - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):393-393.
  • Classical conditioning and the placebo effect.Ian Wickram - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):160-161.
  • Negation in Skinner's system.N. E. Wetherick - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):606-607.
  • Classical conditioning: A manifestation of Bayesian neural learning.James Christopher Westland & Manfred Kochen - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):160-160.
  • Cognition as self–organizing process.Gerhard Werner - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):183-183.
  • Percepts, intervening variables, and neural mechanisms.Wally Welker - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):405-406.
  • Logical atomism and computation do not refute Gibson.Walter B. Welmer - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):405-405.
  • Cognition is not computation, for the reasons that computers don't solve the mind-body problems.Walter B. Weimer - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):152-153.
  • Is extension to perception of real-world objects and scenes possible?J. Wagemans, K. Verfaillie, P. De Graef & K. Lamberts - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):415-417.
  • Adaptive resonance theory: Problems with prediction.Mark Wagner - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (4):675.
  • In defense of invariances and higher-order stimuli.K. von Fieandt - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):404-405.
  • Top-down fast-same, and acoustic perception.Rolf Verleger - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):257-258.
  • The egg revealed.William S. Verplanck - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):605-606.
  • Näätänen's auditory model from a visual perspective.Marinus N. Verbaten - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):256-257.
  • Novelty and the P3.Marinus N. Verbaten - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):398.
  • From epistemology to P3-ology.Rolf Verleger - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):399.
  • Has the case been made against the ecumenical view of connectionism?Robert Van Gulick - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):57-58.
  • Internal representations and indeterminacy: A skeptical view.William R. Uttal - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):392-393.
  • Attention and awareness: Using the to-be-ignored evidence.Geoffrey Underwood - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):256-256.
  • Sensory adaptation and mismatch negativity.P. Ullsperger & T. Baldeweg - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):255-256.
  • Perception, information, and computation.S. Ullman - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):408-415.
  • Against direct perception.Shimon Ullman - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):333-81.
    Central to contemporary cognitive science is the notion that mental processes involve computations defined over internal representations. This view stands in sharp contrast to the to visual perception and cognition, whose most prominent proponent has been J.J. Gibson. In the direct theory, perception does not involve computations of any sort; it is the result of the direct pickup of available information. The publication of Gibson's recent book (Gibson 1979) offers an opportunity to examine his approach, and, more generally, to contrast (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   174 citations  
  • Classical conditioning beyond the reflex: An uneasy rebirth.Jaylan Sheila Turkkan - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):161-179.
  • Classical conditioning: The new hegemony.Jaylan Sheila Turkkan - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):121-137.
    Converging data from different disciplines are showing the role of classical conditioning processes in the elaboration of human and animal behavior to be larger than previously supposed. Restricted views of classically conditioned responses as merely secretory, reflexive, or emotional are giving way to a broader conception that includes problem-solving, and other rule-governed behavior thought to be the exclusive province of either operant conditiońing or cognitive psychology. These new views have been accompanied by changes in the way conditioning is conducted and (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   57 citations  
  • Learning is critical, not implementation versus algorithm.James T. Townsend - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):497-497.
  • On the proper treatment of thermostats.David S. Touretzky - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):55-56.
    A set of hypotheses is formulated for a connectionist approach to cognitive modeling. These hypotheses are shown to be incompatible with the hypotheses underlying traditional cognitive models. The connectionist models considered are massively parallel numerical computational systems that are a kind of continuous dynamical system. The numerical variables in the system correspond semantically to fine-grained features below the level of the concepts consciously used to describe the task domain. The level of analysis is intermediate between those of symbolic cognitive models (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Connectionist models are also algorithmic.David S. Touretzky - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):496-497.
  • Early or late selection? Still an open issue.Steven P. Tipper - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):255-255.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Chaos can be overplayed.René Thom - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):182-183.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Animal versus human minds.H. S. Terrace - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):391-392.
  • What is the algorithmic level?M. M. Taylor & R. A. Pigeau - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):495-496.
  • Temporal processing as related to hemispheric specialization for speech perception in normal and language impaired populations.Paula Tallal - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):77-78.
  • Is the tag necessary?Ron Sun & Emmanuel Schalit - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):415-415.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark