Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Separating the issues involved in the role of bodily movement in perception and perceptual-motor coordination.Robert B. Welch - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):85-86.
  • Three functions of motor-sensory feedback in object perception.Hans Wallach - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):84-85.
  • Face Distortion Aftereffects in Personally Familiar, Famous, and Unfamiliar Faces.Billy Ronald Peter Walton & Peter James Hills - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Attentional factors in depth perception.Richard D. Walk - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):83-84.
  • Visuomotor feedback: A short supplement to Gyr's journey around a polka-dotted cylinder.J. Jacques Vonèche - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):83-83.
  • The thesis of the efference-mediation of vision cannot be rationalized.M. T. Turvey - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):81-83.
  • Are high-level aftereffects perceptual?Katherine R. Storrs - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Methodological considerations in replicating Held and Rekosh's perceptual adaptation study.Martin J. Steinbach - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):81-81.
  • Seeing Goal-Directedness: A Case for Social Perception.Joulia Smortchkova - 2020 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 71 (3):855-879.
    This article focuses on social perception, an area of research that lies at the interface between the philosophy of perception and the scientific investigation of human social cognition. Some philosophers and psychologists appeal to resonance mechanisms to show that intentional and goal-directed actions can be perceived. Against these approaches, I show that there is a class of simple goal-directed actions, whose perception does not rely on resonance. I discuss the role of the superior temporal sulcus as the possible neural correlate (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Does empirical evidence support perceptual mindreading?Joulia Smortchkova - 2020 - Thought: A Journal of Philosophy 9 (4):298-306.
    According to perceptual accounts of mindreading, we can see, rather than cognize, other people's mental states. On one version of this approach, certain mental properties figure in the contents of our perceptual experiences. In a recent paper, Varga has appealed to empirical research to argue that intentions and emotions can indeed be seen, rather than cognized. In this paper, I argue that none of the evidence adduced to support the perceptual account of mindreading shows that we see mental properties, as (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • After-effects and the reach of perceptual content.Joulia Smortchkova - 2020 - Synthese 198 (8):7871-7890.
    In this paper, I discuss the use of after-effects as a criterion for showing that we can perceive high-level properties. According to this criterion, if a high-level property is susceptible to after-effects, this suggests that the property can be perceived, rather than cognized. The defenders of the criterion claim that, since after-effects are also present for low-level, uncontroversially perceptual properties, we can safely infer that high-level after-effects are perceptual as well. The critics of the criterion, on the other hand, assimilate (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Motor system changes are not necessary for changes in perception.George Singer, Meredith Wallace & John K. Collins - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):80-81.
  • Oculomotor hysteresis: implications for testing sensorimotor and ecological optics theories.Wayne L. Shebilske - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):80-80.
  • Parallel Adaptation to Spatially Distinct Distortions.Yannick Sauer, Siegfried Wahl & Katharina Rifai - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Optical distortions as a visual disturbance are inherent in many optical devices such as spectacles or virtual reality headsets. In such devices, distortions vary spatially across the visual field. In progressive addition lenses, for example, the left and right regions of the lens skew the peripheral parts of the wearers visual field in opposing directions. The human visual system adapts to homogeneous distortions and the respective aftereffects are transferred to non-retinotopic locations. This study investigates simultaneous adaptation to two opposing distortions (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Importance of Formalizing Computational Models of Face Adaptation Aftereffects.David A. Ross & Thomas J. Palmeri - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Voluntary movement and perception in intrapersonal and extrapersonal space.P. E. Roland - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):79-80.
  • The problem of adaptation to prismatically-altered shape.Irvin Rock - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):78-79.
  • Adaptation to implied tilt: extensive spatial extrapolation of orientation gradients.Neil W. Roach & Ben S. Webb - 2013 - Frontiers in Psychology 4.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Re-afference in space and movement perception.Austin H. Riesen - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):78-78.
  • Attention as an explanatory concept in perceptual adaptation.Gordon M. Redding - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):77-78.
  • Behavior is sensible but not globally optimal: Seeking common ground in the optimality debate.Dobromir Rahnev & Rachel N. Denison - 2018 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 41.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Position information versus motor programs: two levels of sensorimotor theory.Kenneth R. Paap - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):77-77.
  • The neural basis of visual object learning.Hans P. Op de Beeck & Chris I. Baker - 2010 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14 (1):22-30.
  • Centrifugal contributions to visual perceptual after effects.K. S. K. Murthy - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):77-77.
  • The physiology and psychophysics of the color-form relationship: a review.Konstantinos Moutoussis - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Visual-motor conflict resolved by motor adaptation without perceptual change.Joel M. Miller - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):76-76.
  • Adaptation of the distortion of shape is different from adaptation to the distortion of space.H. H. Mikaelian - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):76-76.
  • Non-Visual Determinants of Perception.Arien Mack - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):75-76.
  • Visual duration aftereffect is position invariant.Baolin Li, Xiangyong Yuan, Youguo Chen, Peiduo Liu & Xiting Huang - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • An Adaptation-Induced Repulsion Illusion in Tactile Spatial Perception.Lux Li, Arielle Chan, Shah M. Iqbal & Daniel Goldreich - 2017 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 11.
  • The encoding of spatial position in the brain.Joseph S. Lappin - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):74-75.
  • A provisional sensory/motor “complementarity” model for adaptation effects.Ivo Kohler - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):73-74.
  • Bodies adapt orientation-independent face representations.Ellyanna Kessler, Shawn A. Walls & Avniel S. Ghuman - 2013 - Frontiers in Psychology 4.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Motor-sensory feedback formulations: are we asking the right questions?J. A. Scott Kelso - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):72-73.
  • Adaptation improves face trustworthiness discrimination.B. D. Keefe, M. Dzhelyova, D. I. Perrett & N. E. Barraclough - 2013 - Frontiers in Psychology 4.
    Direct download (10 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The emotional adaptation aftereffect discriminates between individuals with high and low levels of depressive symptoms.Nan Jiang, Huiling Li, Chuansheng Chen, Ruilin Fu, Yuzhou Zhang & Leilei Mei - 2022 - Cognition and Emotion 36 (2):240-253.
    The adaptation aftereffect plays a critical role in human development and survival. Existing studies have found that, compared with general individuals, individuals with learning disability, autism and dyslexia show a smaller amount of non-affective-based cognitive adaptation aftereffect. Nevertheless, it is unclear whether individuals with depression or depression tendency show similar phenomenon in the adaptation aftereffect, and whether such depression tendency occurs in the non-affective-based cognitive or emotional adaptation aftereffect. To address this question, the present study conducted two experiments. Experiments 1A (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Visuomotor experiments: Failure to replicate, or failure to match the theory?Marc Jeannerod - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):71-71.
  • An analysis of angle, orientation, and location distortions in the bent line aftereffect.Roger B. Howard, Steve R. MacPeek & Charles Byrum - 1975 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 5 (3):233-235.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is there curvature adaptation not attributable to purely intravisual phenomena?Julian Hochberg & Leon Festinger - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):71-71.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Can the brain be divided into a sensory and a motor part?Volker Henn - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):70-71.
  • Interdependent Mechanisms for Processing Gender and Emotion: The Special Status of Angry Male Faces.Daniel A. Harris & Vivian M. Ciaramitaro - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Evaluating nonreplication: more theory and background necessary.Lewis O. Harvey - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):70-70.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • When is sensory-motor information necessary, when only useful, and when superfluous?Ralph Norman Haber - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):68-70.
  • Motor factors in perception.John Gyr, Richmond Willey & Adele Henry - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):86-94.
  • Reference repulsion is not a perceptual illusion.Matthias Fritsche & Floris P. de Lange - 2019 - Cognition 184 (C):107-118.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Motor factors in perception: Limitations in empirical and hierarchical analysis.David Freides - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):68-68.
  • Nonrandom curvature adaptation to random visual displays.Ronald A. Finke - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):68-68.
  • Insufficiencies in perceptual adaptation theory.Sheldon M. Ebenholtz - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):67-68.
  • On the hierarchical inheritance of aftereffects in the visual system.J. Edwin Dickinson & David R. Badcock - 2013 - Frontiers in Psychology 4.
  • What is self-induced motor activity adapting to?R. H. Day - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):66-67.