Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Synchronization and cognitive carpentry: From systematic structuring to simple reasoning. E. Koerner - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):465-466.
  • A Formal Mathematical Model of Cognitive Radio.Ramy A. Fathy, Ahmed A. Abdel-Hafez & Abd El-Halim A. Zekry - 2013 - International Journal of Computer and Information Technology 2 (4).
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rethinking Human and Machine Intelligence through Determinism.Jae Jeong Lee - manuscript
    This paper proposes a metaphysical framework for distinguishing between human and machine intelligence. It posits two identical deterministic worlds -- one comprising a human agent and the other a machine agent. These agents exhibit different information processing mechanisms despite their apparent sameness in a causal sense. Providing a conceptual modeling of their difference, this paper resolves what it calls “the vantage point problem” – namely, how to justify an omniscient perspective through which a determinist asserts determinism from within the supposedly (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The nature of concepts.Denny E. Bradshaw - 1992 - Philosophical Papers 21 (1):1-20.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Cortical connections and parallel processing: Structure and function.Dana H. Ballard - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):67-90.
    The cerebral cortex is a rich and diverse structure that is the basis of intelligent behavior. One of the deepest mysteries of the function of cortex is that neural processing times are only about one hundred times as fast as the fastest response times for complex behavior. At the very least, this would seem to indicate that the cortex does massive amounts of parallel computation.This paper explores the hypothesis that an important part of the cortex can be modeled as a (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   106 citations  
  • What's in the term connectionist?.Christof Koch - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):100-101.
  • Value units make the right connections.Dana H. Ballard - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):107-120.
    The cerebral cortex is a rich and diverse structure that is the basis of intelligent behavior. One of the deepest mysteries of the function of cortex is that neural processing times are only about one hundred times as fast as the fastest response times for complex behavior. At the very least, this would seem to indicate that the cortex does massive amounts of parallel computation.This paper explores the hypothesis that an important part of the cortex can be modeled as a (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Putting together connectionism – again.Paul Smolensky - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):59-74.
    A set of hypotheses is formulated for a connectionist approach to cognitive modeling. These hypotheses are shown to be incompatible with the hypotheses underlying traditional cognitive models. The connectionist models considered are massively parallel numerical computational systems that are a kind of continuous dynamical system. The numerical variables in the system correspond semantically to fine-grained features below the level of the concepts consciously used to describe the task domain. The level of analysis is intermediate between those of symbolic cognitive models (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Theory autonomy and future promise.Matti Sintonen - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):488-488.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Toward a theory of human memory: Data structures and access processes.Michael S. Humphreys, Janet Wiles & Simon Dennis - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):655-667.
    Starting from Marr's ideas about levels of explanation, a theory of the data structures and access processes in human memory is demonstrated on 10 tasks. Functional characteristics of human memory are captured implementation-independently. Our theory generates a multidimensional task classification subsuming existing classifications such as the distinction between tasks that are implicit versus explicit, data driven versus conceptually driven, and simple associative (two-way bindings) versus higher order (threeway bindings), providing a broad basis for new experiments. The formal language clarifies the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   69 citations  
  • Rethinking Human and Machine Intelligence through Kant, Wittgenstein, Gödel, and Cantor.Lee Jae Jeong - manuscript
    This paper proposes a new metaphysical framework for distinguishing between human and machine intelligence by drawing on Kant’s incongruent counterparts as an analogy. Specifically, the paper posits two deterministic worlds that are superficially identical but ultimately different. Using ideas from Wittgenstein, Gödel, and Cantor, the paper defines “deterministic knowledge” and investigates how this knowledge is processed differently in those two worlds. The paper considers computationalism and causal determinism for the new framework. Then, the paper introduces new concepts to illustrate why (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Texting ECHO on historical data.Jan M. Zytkow - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):489-490.
  • Modularity need not imply locality: Damaged modules can have nonlocal effects.Edgar Zurif & David Swinney - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):89-90.
  • Does connectionism suffice?Steven W. Zucker - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):301-302.
  • Adaptation and attention.Steven W. Zucker - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):458-458.
  • What counts as local?Andrew W. Young - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):88-89.
  • Ethereal oscillations.Malcolm P. Young - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):476-477.
  • The reality of the symbolic and subsymbolic systems.Andrew Woodfield & Adam Morton - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):58-58.
  • Complexity, guided search, and the data.Jeremy M. Wolfe - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):457-458.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cartesian vs. Newtonian research strategies for cognitive science.Morton E. Winston - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):463-464.
  • Story grammars versus story points.Robert Wilensky - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (4):579.
  • Point: Counterpoint.Robert Wilensky - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (4):613.
  • Psychology, or sociology of science?N. E. Wetherick - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):489-489.
  • Cognition and simulation.N. E. Wetherick - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):462-463.
  • Cognition as self–organizing process.Gerhard Werner - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):183-183.
  • Connectionist learning and the challenge of real environments.Mark Weaver & Stephen Kaplan - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):510-511.
  • The localization/distribution distinction in neuropsychology is related to the isomorphism/multiple meaning distinction in cell electrophysiology.Gerald S. Wasserman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):87-88.
  • The holes in points.David L. Waltz & Marcy H. Dorfman - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (4):612.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Massively Parallel Parsing: A Strongly Interactive Model of Natural Language Interpretation.David L. Waltz & Jordan B. Pollack - 1985 - Cognitive Science 9 (1):51-74.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   95 citations  
  • On putting the cart before the horse: Taking perception seriously in unified theories of cognition.Kim J. Vicente & Alex Kirlik - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):461-462.
  • A cognitive process shell.Steven A. Vere - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):460-461.
  • The symbolic brain or the invisible hand?René van Hezewijk & Edward H. F. de Haan - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):85-86.
  • Playing Flourens to Fodor's Gall.Tim van Gelder - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):84-84.
  • Prosopagnosia, conscious awareness and the interactive brain.Robert Van Gulick - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):84-85.
  • Marr versus Marr: On the notion of levels.Frank van der Velde, Gezinus Wolters & A. H. C. van der Heijden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):681-682.
  • Has the case been made against the ecumenical view of connectionism?Robert Van Gulick - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):57-58.
  • Connectionist models learn what?Timothy van Gelder - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):509-510.
  • A pointless approach to stories.Teun A. van Dijk - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (4):598.
  • On brains and models.William R. Uttal - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):456-457.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On models and mechanisms.William R. Uttal - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):459-460.
  • The functional architecture of visual attention may still be modular.Carlo Umiltà - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):82-83.
  • Some important constraints on complexity.Leonard Uhr - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):455-456.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Dynamic-binding theory is not plausible without chaotic oscillation.Ichiro Tsuda - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):475-476.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Modeling visual attention via selective tuning.John K. Tsotsos, Scan M. Culhane, Winky Yan Kei Wai, Yuzhong Lai, Neal Davis & Fernando Nuflo - 1995 - Artificial Intelligence 78 (1-2):507-545.
  • Connectionist computing and neural machinery: Examining the test of “timing”.John K. Tsotsos - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):106-107.
  • Analyzing vision at the complexity level.John K. Tsotsos - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):423-445.
    The general problem of visual search can be shown to be computationally intractable in a formal, complexity-theoretic sense, yet visual search is extensively involved in everyday perception, and biological systems manage to perform it remarkably well. Complexity level analysis may resolve this contradiction. Visual search can be reshaped into tractability through approximations and by optimizing the resources devoted to visual processing. Architectural constraints can be derived using the minimum cost principle to rule out a large class of potential solutions. The (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   154 citations  
  • Complexity Level Analysis Revisited: What Can 30 Years of Hindsight Tell Us about How the Brain Might Represent Visual Information?John K. Tsotsos - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A little complexity analysis goes a long way.John K. Tsotsos - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):458-469.
  • Search and the detection and integration of features.Anne Treisman - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):454-455.
  • Unified theories and theories that mimic each other's predictions.James T. Townsend - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):458-459.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation