Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The return of the reinforcement theorists.C. D. L. Wynne - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):156-156.
  • A mathematical theory of reinforcement: An unexpected place to find support for analogical memory coding.Donald M. Wilkie & Lisa M. Saksida - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):155-156.
  • Fifty years on: The new “principles of behavior”?J. H. Wearden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):155-155.
  • Connectionist learning and the challenge of real environments.Mark Weaver & Stephen Kaplan - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):510-511.
  • How general is a general theory of reinforcement?Stephen F. Walker - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):154-155.
  • Smolensky's theory of mind.Paul F. M. J. Verschure - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):407-407.
  • On observing emergent properties and their compositions.Francisco T. Varela & Vicente Sanchez-Leighton - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):401-402.
  • Connectionist models learn what?Timothy van Gelder - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):509-510.
  • Advances in neural network theory.Gérard Toulouse - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):509-509.
  • Connectionist models: Too little too soon?William Timberlake - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):508-509.
  • Animal-centered models of reinforcement.William Timberlake - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):153-154.
  • Crossword expertise as recognitional decision making: an artificial intelligence approach.Kejkaew Thanasuan & Shane T. Mueller - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Short-term memory in human operant conditioning.Frode Svartdal - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):152-153.
  • Where do Bayesian priors come from?Patrick Suppes - 2007 - Synthese 156 (3):441-471.
    Bayesian prior probabilities have an important place in probabilistic and statistical methods. In spite of this fact, the analysis of where these priors come from and how they are formed has received little attention. It is reasonable to excuse the lack, in the foundational literature, of detailed psychological theory of what are the mechanisms by which prior probabilities are formed. But it is less excusable that there is an almost total absence of a detailed discussion of the highly differentiating nature (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Problems of extension, representation, and computational irreducibility.Patrick Suppes - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):507-508.
  • The scale of nature: Fitted parameters and dimensional correctness.D. W. Stephens - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):150-152.
  • Absolute Identification by Relative Judgment.Neil Stewart, Gordon D. A. Brown & Nick Chater - 2005 - Psychological Review 112 (4):881-911.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  • How automatic are crossmodal correspondences?Charles Spence & Ophelia Deroy - 2013 - Consciousness and Cognition 22 (1):245-260.
    The last couple of years have seen a rapid growth of interest in the study of crossmodal correspondences – the tendency for our brains to preferentially associate certain features or dimensions of stimuli across the senses. By now, robust empirical evidence supports the existence of numerous crossmodal correspondences, affecting people’s performance across a wide range of psychological tasks – in everything from the redundant target effect paradigm through to studies of the Implicit Association Test, and from speeded discrimination/classification tasks through (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • In defense of PTC.Paul Smolensky - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):407-412.
  • Learning to signal with probe and adjust.Brian Skyrms - 2012 - Episteme 9 (2):139-150.
    This is an investigation of the emergence of signaling using one kind of trial and error learning: probe and adjust.Send article to KindleTo send this article to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about sending to your Kindle. Find out more about (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Stimulus awareness is necessary for both instrumental learning and instrumental responding to previously learned stimuli.Lina I. Skora, Ryan B. Scott & Gerhard Jocham - 2024 - Cognition 244 (C):105716.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Practical effects of response specification.Richard L. Shull - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):150-150.
  • Awareness and reinforcement.Charles P. Shimp - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):149-150.
  • There is more to learning then meeth the eye.Noel E. Sharkey - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):506-507.
  • Models and reality.John R. Searle - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):399-399.
  • Evidence for Learning to Learn Behavior in Normal Form Games.Timothy C. Salmon - 2004 - Theory and Decision 56 (4):367-404.
    Evidence presented in Salmon (2001; Econometrica 69(6) 1597) indicates that typical tests to identify learning behavior in experiments involving normal form games possess little power to reject incorrect models. This paper begins by presenting results from an experiment designed to gather alternative data to overcome this problem. The results from these experiments indicate support for a learning-to-learn or rule learning hypothesis in which subjects change their decision rule over time. These results are then used to construct an adaptive learning model (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • How persuasive is a good fit? A comment on theory testing.Seth Roberts & Harold Pashler - 2000 - Psychological Review 107 (2):358-367.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   86 citations  
  • Perspectives on Modeling in Cognitive Science.Richard M. Shiffrin - 2010 - Topics in Cognitive Science 2 (4):736-750.
    This commentary gives a personal perspective on modeling and modeling developments in cognitive science, starting in the 1950s, but focusing on the author’s personal views of modeling since training in the late 1960s, and particularly focusing on advances since the official founding of the Cognitive Science Society. The range and variety of modeling approaches in use today are remarkable, and for many, bewildering. Yet to come to anything approaching adequate insights into the infinitely complex fields of mind, brain, and intelligent (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Regularity Extraction Across Species: Associative Learning Mechanisms Shared by Human and Non‐Human Primates.Arnaud Rey, Laure Minier, Raphaëlle Malassis, Louisa Bogaerts & Joël Fagot - 2019 - Topics in Cognitive Science 11 (3):573-586.
    One of the themes that has been widely addressed in both the implicit learning and statistical learning literatures is that of rule learning. While it is widely agreed that the extraction of regularities from the environment is a fundamental facet of cognition, there is still debate about the nature of rule learning. Rey and colleagues show that the comparison between human and non‐human primates can contribute important insights to this debate.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Level of analysis is not a central issue.James A. Reggia - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):406-407.
  • Memory and the integration of response sequences.Phil Reed - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):148-149.
  • The analysis of the learning needs to be deeper.John E. Rager - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):505-506.
  • From overt behavior to hypothetical behavior to memory: Inference in the wrong direction.Howard Rachlin - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):147-148.
  • Information processing and the decremental effect of intermittent reinforcement schedules in human conditioning.William F. Prokasy & William C. Williams - 1979 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 14 (1):57-60.
  • Effects of partial and continuous reinforcement on acquisition and extinction in classical appetitive conditioning.C. X. Poulos & I. Gormezano - 1974 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 4 (3):197-198.
  • Realistic neural nets need to learn iconic representations.W. A. Phillips, P. J. B. Hancock & L. S. Smith - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):505-505.
  • Problems and pitfalls for Killeen's mathematical principles of reinforcement.Joseph J. Pear - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):146-147.
  • Learning from learned networks.M. Pavel - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):503-504.
  • Connectionism: Self-abuse is improper treatment.Gregg C. Oden - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):402-402.
  • Extension to multiple schedules: Some surprising (and accurate) predictions.John A. Nevin - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):145-146.
  • Keeping representations at bay.Stanley Munsat - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):502-503.
  • Killeen's theory provides an answer – and a question.Mary Ann Metzger & Terje Sagvolden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):144-145.
  • Evolution and connectionism.Neil McNaughton - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):402-403.
  • The psychology of connectionism.Dominic W. Massaro - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):403-406.
  • Toward a unification of conditioning and cognition in animal learning.William S. Maki - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):501-502.
  • Why Higher Working Memory Capacity May Help You Learn: Sampling, Search, and Degrees of Approximation.Kevin Lloyd, Adam Sanborn, David Leslie & Stephan Lewandowsky - 2019 - Cognitive Science 43 (12):e12805.
    Algorithms for approximate Bayesian inference, such as those based on sampling (i.e., Monte Carlo methods), provide a natural source of models of how people may deal with uncertainty with limited cognitive resources. Here, we consider the idea that individual differences in working memory capacity (WMC) may be usefully modeled in terms of the number of samples, or “particles,” available to perform inference. To test this idea, we focus on two recent experiments that report positive associations between WMC and two distinct (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • On learnability, empirical foundations, and naturalness.W. J. M. Levelt - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):501-501.
  • Memories and functional response units.Kennon A. Lattal & Josele Abreu-Rodrigues - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):143-144.
  • Approaches to learning and representation.Pat Langley - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):500-501.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What can psychologists learn from hidden-unit nets?K. Lamberts & G. D'Ydewalle - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):499-500.