Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Velmans's overfocused perspective on consciousness.Marcel Kinsbourne - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):682-683.
  • Strong inferences about hypnosis.John F. Kihlstrom - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):474-475.
  • Availability, accessibility, and subliminal perception.John F. Kihlstrom - 2004 - Consciousness and Cognition 13 (1):92-100.
  • The construction of Subjective Experience: Memory Attributions.Colleen M. Kelley & Larry L. Jacoby - 1990 - Mind and Language 5 (1):49-68.
  • The construction of subjective experience: Memory attributions.Clarence M. Kelley & Larry L. Jacoby - 1990 - Mind and Language 5 (1):49-68.
  • Consciousness, analogy and creativity.Mark T. Keane - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):682-682.
  • Semantic activation, consciousness, and attention.William A. Johnston - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):35-36.
  • The where in the brain determines the when in the mind.M. Jeannerod - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):212-213.
  • Hypnosis: Artichoke or onion?Richard St Jean - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):482-482.
  • Introspective physicalism as an approach to the science of consciousness.Anthony I. Jack & T. Shallice - 2001 - Cognition 79 (1):161-196.
    Most ?theories of consciousness? are based on vague speculations about the properties of conscious experience. We aim to provide a more solid basis for a science of consciousness. We argue that a theory of consciousness should provide an account of the very processes that allow us to acquire and use information about our own mental states ? the processes underlying introspection. This can be achieved through the construction of information processing models that can account for ?Type-C? processes. Type-C processes can (...)
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   82 citations  
  • Explaining what?Elizabeth Irvine - unknown
    The Hard Problem is surrounded by a vast literature, to which it is increasingly hard to contribute to in any meaningful way. Accordingly, the strategy here is not to offer any new metaphysical or ‘in principle’ arguments in favour of the success of materialism, but to assume a Type Q(uinian) approach and look to contemporary consciousness science to see how the concept of consciousness fares there, and what kind of explanations we can hope to offer of it. It is suggested (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Attentional orienting precedes conscious identification.Albrecht Werner Inhoff - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):35-35.
  • Limits of preconscious processing.Albrecht Werner Inhoff - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):680-681.
  • Some mistakes about consciousness and their motivation.S. L. Hurley - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):211-212.
  • Semantic activation without conscious identification: Can progress be made?Daniel Holender - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):768.
  • Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal.Daniel Holender - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):1-23.
    When the stored representation of the meaning of a stimulus is accessed through the processing of a sensory input it is maintained in an activated state for a certain amount of time that allows for further processing. This semantic activation is generally accompanied by conscious identification, which can be demonstrated by the ability of a person to perform discriminations on the basis of the meaning of the stimulus. The idea that a sensory input can give rise to semantic activation without (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   447 citations  
  • Conceptual, experimental, and theoretical indeterminacies in research on semantic activation without conscious identification.Daniel Holender - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):50-66.
  • Unconscious perception revisited: A comment on Merikle (1992).S. H. A. Henley - 1984 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 22 (2):121-4.
  • Unconscious perception re-revisited: A comment on Merikle’s paper.S. H. A. Henley - 1984 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 22 (2):121-124.
  • Dreaming, cognition, and physical illness: Part I.Robert Haskell - 1985 - Journal of Medical Humanities and Bioethics 6 (1):46-56.
    Part I of a two part article on the effect upon dreaming on physical illness briefly explores the historical medico-philosophical antecedents of the notion that dreams can be diagnostic of bodily disease. Modern sleep research findings relating REM sleep to physiologic changes are also explored. The controversy of whether dreams are merely the consequence of random brain activity or whether they are a valid psychological phenomenon is discussed. Six contemporary views of the function of REM sleep are outlined. A seventh (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Epiphenomenalism and the reduction of experience.Valerie Gray Hardcastle - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):680-680.
  • The two visual system hypothesis loses a supporter.Ralph Norman Haber - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):453.
  • On relativity of time and the causal status of consciousness.Ali Habibi & Michael S. Bendele - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):404-405.
  • Consciousness may still have a processing role to play.Robert Van Gulick - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):699-700.
  • On not knowing the meanings of words we can detect: Crucial qualitative differences.J. A. Groeger - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):765.
  • Nothing is instantaneous, even in sensation.Robert A. M. Gregson - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):210-211.
  • Has consciousness a sharp edge?Robert A. M. Gregson - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):679-680.
  • What is the relation between language and consciousness?Jeffrey A. Gray - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):679-679.
  • Explaining “virtuoso” hypnotic performance: Social psychology or experiential skill?Kenneth R. Graham - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):473-474.
  • The Cartesian Theater stance.Bruce Glymour, Rick Grush, Valerie Gray Hardcastle, Brian Keeley, Joe Ramsey, Oron Shagrir & Ellen Watson - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):209-210.
  • Putting consciousness in a box: Once more around the track.Joseph Glicksohn - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):404-404.
  • Memory with and without recollective experience.John M. Gardiner - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):678-679.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • An operational definition of conscious awareness must be responsible to subjective experience.Carol A. Fowler - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):33-35.
  • Dream processing.David Foulkes - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):678-678.
  • Knowing and knowing you know: Better methods or better models?Ira Fischler - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):32-33.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The distributed pineal gland.Martha J. Farah - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):209-209.
  • Identification, masking, and priming: Clarifying the issues.Lindsay J. Evett, Glyn W. Humphreys & Philip T. Quinlan - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):31-32.
  • Hypnosis and behavioral compliance: Is the cup half-empty or half-full?Frederick J. Evans - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):471-473.
  • The standing of psychoanalysis.Edward Erwin - 1984 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 35 (2):115-128.
    tries to elucidate some of the rational considerations that determine the standing and value of psychoanalysis. He is sceptical about much of the positive evidence, but he also tries to provide some support for Freudian doctrines. I examine his supporting arguments and try to show that they have serious weaknesses.
    Direct download (10 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Experimental indeterminacies in the dissociation paradigm of subliminal perception.Matthew Hugh Erdelyi - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):30-31.
  • Hypnosis and social suggestibility.William E. Edmonston - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):470-471.
  • Observing protocol.Judith Economos - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):677-677.
  • Direct awareness and inference.Judith Economos - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):452.
  • Functional consequences of perceiving facial expressions of emotion without awareness.John D. Eastwood & Daniel Smilek - 2005 - Consciousness and Cognition 14 (3):565-584.
    A substantial body of research has established that even when we are not consciously aware of the faces of others we are nevertheless sensitive to, and impacted by their facial expression. In this paper, we consider this body of research from a new perspective by examining the functions of unconscious perception revealed by these studies. A consideration of the literature from this perspective highlights that existing research methods are limited when it comes to revealing possible functions of unconscious perception. The (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Conscious acts and their objects.Fred Dretske - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):676-677.
  • Replicable unconscious semantic priming.Sean Draine & Anthony G. Greenwald - 1998 - Journal Of Experimental Psychology-General 127 (3):286-303.
  • On private events and brain events.Norman F. Dixon - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):29-30.
  • Hydrocephalus and “misapplied competence”: Awkward evidence for or against?N. F. Dixon - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):675-676.
  • Time and the observer: The where and when of consciousness in the brain.Daniel C. Dennett & Marcel Kinsbourne - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):183-201.
    _Behavioral and Brain Sciences_ , 15, 183-247, 1992. Reprinted in _The Philosopher's Annual_ , Grim, Mar and Williams, eds., vol. XV-1992, 1994, pp. 23-68; Noel Sheehy and Tony Chapman, eds., _Cognitive Science_ , Vol. I, Elgar, 1995, pp.210-274.
    Direct download (12 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   371 citations  
  • Escape from the Cartesian Theater.Daniel C. Dennett & Marcel Kinsbourne - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):234-247.