Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. On one as an anaphor.Stephen Neale - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):353-354.
  • The child's trigger experience: Degree-0 learnability.David Lightfoot - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):321-334.
    According to a “selective” (as opposed to “instructive”) model of human language capacity, people come to know more than they experience. The discrepancy between experience and eventual capacity (the “poverty of the stimulus”) is bridged by genetically provided information. Hence any hypothesis about the linguistic genotype (or “Universal Grammar,” UG) has consequences for what experience is needed and what form people's mature capacities (or “grammars”) will take. This BBS target article discusses the “trigger experience,” that is, the experience that actually (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   93 citations  
  • Procedural Semantics for Hyperintensional Logic: Foundations and Applications of Transparent Intensional Logic.Marie Duží, Bjorn Jespersen & Pavel Materna - 2010 - Dordrecht, Netherland: Springer.
    The book is about logical analysis of natural language. Since we humans communicate by means of natural language, we need a tool that helps us to understand in a precise manner how the logical and formal mechanisms of natural language work. Moreover, in the age of computers, we need to communicate both with and through computers as well. Transparent Intensional Logic is a tool that is helpful in making our communication and reasoning smooth and precise. It deals with all kinds (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   50 citations  
  • Natural Genome Editing from a Biocommunicative Perspective.Guenther Witzany - 2011 - Biosemiotics 4 (3):349-368.
    Natural genome editing from a biocommunicative perspective is the competent agent-driven generation and integration of meaningful nucleotide sequences into pre-existing genomic content arrangements, and the ability to (re-)combine and (re-)regulate them according to context-dependent (i.e. adaptational) purposes of the host organism. Natural genome editing integrates both natural editing of genetic code and epigenetic marking that determines genetic reading patterns. As agents that edit genetic code and epigenetically mark genomic structures, viral and subviral agents have been suggested because they may be (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Why degree-0?Wendy Wilkins - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):362-363.
  • Linguistic variation and learnability.Edwin Williams - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):363-364.
  • Why degree-0?Thomas Wasow - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):361-362.
  • Cognitive maps: dimensionality and development.J. Jacques Vonèche - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):519-520.
  • Observing obsolescence.Nigel Vincent - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):360-361.
  • Is hippocampal rhythmical slow activity specifically related to movement through space?C. M. Vanderwolf - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):518-519.
  • Anatomical units in psychology.Holger Ursin - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):518-518.
  • “Model systems” versus “neuroethological” approach to hippocampal function.Richard F. Thompson, Paul R. Solomon & Donald J. Weisz - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):517-518.
  • Cognition, memory, and the hippocampus.Garth J. Thomas - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):515-517.
  • The anatomy of a cognitive map.L. W. Swanson - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):515-515.
  • What every speaker cognizes.Stephen P. Stich - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):39-40.
  • What's a trigger?Edward P. Stabler - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):358-360.
  • The hippocampus, space, and human amnesia.Larry R. Squire - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):514-515.
  • Representation and psychological reality.Elliott Sober - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):38-39.
    In this brief space I want to describe how Chomsky's analysis of "psychological reality" departs from what I think is a fairly standard construal of the idea. This familiar formulation arises from distinguishing between someone's following a rule and someone's acting in conformity with a rule. The former idea, but not the latter, involves the idea that the person has some mental representation of the rule that plays a certain causal role in determining behavior. Although there may be many grammatical (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Data on language input: Incomprehensible omission indeed!Catherine E. Snow & Michael Tomasello - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):357-358.
  • Rules and causation.John R. Searle - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):37-38.
  • Language acquisition: Dubious assumptions and a specious explanatory principle.I. M. Schlesinger - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):355-356.
  • An artificial intelligence perspective on Chomsky's view of language.Roger C. Schank - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):35-37.
  • Chomsky's evidence against Chomsky's theory.Geoffrey Sampson - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):34-35.
  • The modularity and maturation of cognitive capacities.David M. Rosenthal - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):32-34.
  • On the format for parameters.Luigi Rizzi - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):355-356.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On O'Keefe, Nadel, space and brain.James B. Ranck - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):513-514.
  • Cross purposes.Howard Rachlln - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):30-31.
  • The faculty of language: what's special about it?Steven Pinker & Ray Jackendoff - 2005 - Cognition 95 (2):201-236.
  • Mental maps, mental images, and intuitions about space.Steven Pinker - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):512-512.
  • L'explication en grammaire transformationnelle.Claude Panaccio - 1979 - Dialogue 18 (3):307-341.
    Malgré les critiques de toutes sortes dont elle fut et dont elle est encore l'objet, la grammaire transformationnelle est devenue en vingt ans, sous l'impulsion de Noam Chomsky, le paradigme dominant de la linguistique contemporaine. Et, bien sûr, l'incroyable rapidité de cette ascension s'est accompagnée de nombreuses crises de croissance. Linguistes, philosophes et psychologues, ainsi amenés à s'interroger sur les fondements épistémologiques de cette étrange discipline, se sont heurtés à de redoutables énigmes métathéoriques: de quel domaine factuel la linguistique est-elle (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Inner and outer space: the neuroanatomical bases of spatially organized behaviors.David S. Olton - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):511-512.
  • The cognitive map as a hippocampus.John O'Keefe & Lynn Nadel - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):520-533.
  • Précis of O'Keefe & Nadel's The hippocampus as a cognitive map.John O'Keefe & Lynn Nadel - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):487-494.
    Theories of spatial cognition are derived from many sources. Psychologists are concerned with determining the features of the mind which, in combination with external inputs, produce our spatialized experience. A review of philosophical and other approaches has convinced us that the brain must come equipped to impose a three-dimensional Euclidean framework on experience – our analysis suggests that object re-identification may require such a framework. We identify this absolute, nonegocentric, spatial framework with a specific neural system centered in the hippocampus.A (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   178 citations  
  • Two perspectives on learnability.William O'Grady - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):354-355.
  • The non-uniqueness of semantic solutions: Polysemy. [REVIEW]Geoffrey Nunberg - 1979 - Linguistics and Philosophy 3 (2):143 - 184.
  • Time: a fourth dimension for the hippocampal cognitive map.Arthur J. Nonneman - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):511-511.
  • Structural realism and generative linguistics.Ryan M. Nefdt - 2021 - Synthese 199 (1-2):3711-3737.
    Linguistics as a science has rapidly changed during the course of a relatively short period. The mathematical foundations of the science, however, present a different story below the surface. In this paper, I argue that due to the former, the seismic shifts in theory over the past 80 years opens linguistics up to the problem of pessimistic meta-induction or radical theory change. I further argue that, due to the latter, one current solution to this problem in the philosophy of science, (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Notational Variants and Cognition: The Case of Dependency Grammar.Ryan M. Nefdt & Giosué Baggio - forthcoming - Erkenntnis:1-31.
    In recent years, dependency grammars have established themselves as valuable tools in theoretical and computational linguistics. To many linguists, dependency grammars and the more standard constituency-based formalisms are notational variants. We argue that, beyond considerations of formal equivalence, cognition may also serve as a background for a genuine comparison between these different views of syntax. In this paper, we review and evaluate some of the most common arguments and evidence employed to advocate for the cognitive or neural reality of dependency (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is C hl linguistically specific?Nirmalangshu Mukherji - 2003 - Philosophical Psychology 16 (2):289 – 308.
    C HL is Noam Chomsky's shorthand for "Single Computational System of human language." C HL is that part of the faculty of language (FL) that integrates lexical information to form linguistic expressions at the interfaces where language interacts with other cognitive systems. In this paper, I am asking whether the elements of FL are dedicated to language alone, or whether significant parts of FL might apply beyond language. From a close examination of the properties of the principles of C HL (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • There are many modular theories of mind.Adam Morton - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):29-29.
  • Language: levels of characterisation.John Morton - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):29-30.
  • Learnability considerations and the nature of trigger experiences in language acquisition.James L. Morgan - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):352-353.
  • Chomsky's radical break with modern traditions.Julius M. Moravcsik - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):28-29.
  • The hippocampus and informational salience.John W. Moore - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):510-511.
  • iTabula si, rasa no!James D. McCawley - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):26-27.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • INFL', Spec, and other fabulous beasts.James D. McCawley - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):350-352.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Language learning versus grammar growth.Robert J. Matthews - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):25-26.
  • The new organology.John C. Marshall - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):23-25.
  • The true nature of the linguistic trigger.Marjorie Perlman Lorch - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):350-350.
  • Matching parameters to simple triggers.David Lightfoot - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):364-375.