Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. A review of the literature with and without awareness. [REVIEW]George Wolford - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):49-50.
  • Consciousness: Limited but consequential.Timothy D. Wilson - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):701-701.
  • Models as toothbrushes.Michael J. Watkins - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):86-86.
  • How do representations get processed in real nerve cells?Gerald S. Wasserman - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):85-85.
  • No conscious or co-conscious?Graham F. Wagstaff - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):700-700.
  • Is human information processing conscious?Max Velmans - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):651-69.
    Investigations of the function of consciousness in human information processing have focused mainly on two questions: (1) where does consciousness enter into the information processing sequence and (2) how does conscious processing differ from preconscious and unconscious processing. Input analysis is thought to be initially "preconscious," "pre-attentive," fast, involuntary, and automatic. This is followed by "conscious," "focal-attentive" analysis which is relatively slow, voluntary, and flexible. It is thought that simple, familiar stimuli can be identified preconsciously, but conscious processing is needed (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   346 citations  
  • Consciousness from a first-person perspective.Max Velmans - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):702-726.
    This paper replies to the first 36 commentaries on my target article on “Is human information processing conscious?” (Behavioral and Brain Sciences,1991, pp.651-669). The target article focused largely on experimental studies of how consciousness relates to human information processing, tracing their relation from input through to output, while discussion of the implications of the findings both for cognitive psychology and philosophy of mind was relatively brief. The commentaries reversed this emphasis, and so, correspondingly, did the reply. The sequence of topics (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   131 citations  
  • Facilitation or inhibition from parafoveal words?Geoffrey Underwood - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):48-49.
  • Attention is necessary for word integration.Geoffrey Underwood - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):698-698.
  • Stage models of mental processing and the additive-factor method.Saul Sternberg - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):82-84.
  • Priming without awareness: What was all the fuss about?Keith E. Stanovich & Dean G. Purcell - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):47-48.
  • Damn! There goes that ghost again!Keith E. Stanovich - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):696-698.
  • Dissociating consciousness from cognition.David Spiegel - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):695-696.
  • Developing concepts of consciousness.Aaron Sloman - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):694-695.
  • A lawful first-person psychology involving a causal consciousness: A psychoanalytic solution.Howard Shevrin - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):693-694.
  • Pipelines, processing models, and the mindbody problem.John G. Seamon - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):81-82.
  • Practice, attention, and the processing system.Walter Schneider - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):80-81.
  • Isn't the first-person perspective a bad third-person perspective?W. Schaeken & G. D'Ydewalle - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):692-693.
  • Information-flow diagrams as scientific models.Kenneth M. Sayre - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):79-80.
  • The use of interference paradigms as a criterion for separating memory stores.Henry L. Roediger - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):78-79.
  • A limitation of the reflex-arc approach to consciousness.J. Steven Reznick & Philip David Zelazo - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):692-692.
  • Reasons for doubting the existence of even epiphenomenal consciousness.Georges Rey - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):691-692.
  • Against semantic preprocessing in parafoveal vision.Keith Rayner - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):46-47.
  • Simplistic heuristics and Maltese acrostics.Patrick Rabbitt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):77-78.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The pilfering of awareness and guilt by association.Kenneth R. Paap - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):45-46.
  • Processing of the unattended message during selective dichotic listening.R. Näätänen - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):43-44.
  • The usefulness for memory theory of the word “store”.D. J. Murray - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):76-77.
  • What kind of a framework?John Morton - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):75-76.
  • What do you mean by conscious?John Morton - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):43-43.
  • Consciousness is a “subjective” state.Philip M. Merikle & Jim Cheesman - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):42-42.
  • Semantic activation and reading.George W. McConkie - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):41-42.
  • Memory and mood.Maryanne Martin - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):75-75.
  • Consciousness and processing: Choosing and testing a null hypothesis.Anthony J. Marcel - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):40-41.
  • The processing of information is not conscious, but its products often are.George Mandler - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):688-689.
  • Epi-arguments for epiphenomenalism.Bruce Mangan - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):689-690.
  • The psychophysics of subliminal perception.Neil A. Macmillan - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):38-39.
  • The homunculus as bureaucrat.Alan K. Mackworth - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):74-74.
  • Consciousness is king of the neuronal processors.William A. MacKay - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):687-688.
  • Conscious identification: Where do you draw the line?Stephen J. Lupker - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):37-38.
  • Broadbent's Maltese cross memory model: Something old, something new, something borrowed, something missing.Elizabeth F. Loftus, Geoffrey R. Loftus & Earl B. Hunt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):73-74.
  • Consciousness: Only introspective hindsight?Dan Lloyd - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):686-687.
  • Conscious functions and brain processes.Benjamin Libet - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):685-686.
  • Approaches to consciousness: Psychophysics or philosophy?Richard Latto & John Campion - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):36-37.
  • Understanding awareness at the neuronal level.Christof Koch & Francis Crick - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):683-685.
  • Is consciousness information processing?Raymond Klein - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):683-683.
  • Velmans's overfocused perspective on consciousness.Marcel Kinsbourne - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):682-683.
  • Consciousness, analogy and creativity.Mark T. Keane - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):682-682.
  • Semantic activation, consciousness, and attention.William A. Johnston - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):35-36.