Switch to: References

Citations of:

Mach's philosophy of science

[London]: Athlone Press of the University of London (1971)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. A perspective for viewing the history of psychophysics.David J. Murray - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):115-137.
    Fechner's conception of psychophysics included both “outer psychophysics” the relation between stimulus intensity and the response reflecting sensation strength, and “inner psychophysics” the relation between neurelectric responses and sensation strength. In his own time outer psychophysics focussed on the form of the psychophysical law, with Fechner espousing a logarithmic law, Delboeuf a variant of the logarithmic law incorporating a resting level of neural activity, and Plateau a power law. One of the issues on which the dispute was focussed concerned the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  • Is economics still immersed in the old concepts of the Enlightenment era?Andrzej P. Wierzbicki - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):236-237.
  • A perspective for viewing the present of psychophysics.Paul Whittle - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):165-166.
  • Looking backward: Progress in outer psychophysics.David J. Weiss - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):165-165.
  • On the construction of psychophysical reality.Mark Wagner - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):164-165.
  • The analysis of sensations as the foundation of all sciences.J. van Brakel - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):163-164.
  • Psychophysics and the mind-brain problem.Michel Treisman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):162-163.
  • The human being as a bumbling optimalist: A psychologist's viewpoint.Masanao Toda - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):235-235.
  • Sensation strength: Another point of view.Robert Teghtsoonian - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):161-162.
  • Optimal confusion.Stephanie Stolarz-Fantino & Edmund Fantino - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):234-234.
  • Bedrock metaphysics, fossil fuel psychophysics.Dale A. Stout - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):160-161.
  • Avoid the push-pull dilemma in explanation.Kenneth M. Steele - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):233-234.
  • Extremum descriptions, process laws and minimality heuristics.Elliott Sober - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):232-233.
    The examples and concepts that Shoemaker cites are rather heterogeneous. Some distinctions need to be drawn. An optimality thesis involves not just an ordering of options, but a value judgment about them. So let us begin by distinguishing minimality from optimality. And the concept of minimality can play a variety of roles, among which I distinguish between extremum descriptions, statements hypothesizing an optimizing process, and methodological recommendations. Finally, I consider how the three categories relate to Shoemaker’s question that “Who is (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rational agents, real people and the quest for optimality.Eldar Shafir - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):232-232.
  • The strategy of optimality revisited.Paul J. H. Schoemaker - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):237-245.
  • The quest for optimality: A positive heuristic of science?Paul J. H. Schoemaker - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):205-215.
    This paper examines the strengths and weaknesses of one of science's most pervasive and flexible metaprinciples;optimalityis used to explain utility maximization in economics, least effort principles in physics, entropy in chemistry, and survival of the fittest in biology. Fermat's principle of least time involves both teleological and causal considerations, two distinct modes of explanation resting on poorly understood psychological primitives. The rationality heuristic in economics provides an example from social science of the potential biases arising from the extreme flexibility of (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • Should the quest for optimality worry us?Nils-Eric Sahlin - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):231-231.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Unwarranted popularity of a power function for heaviness estimates.Helen E. Ross - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):159-160.
  • Optimality as a prescriptive tool.Alexander H. G. Rinnooy Kan - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):230-231.
  • Response time based psychophysics: An added perspective.William M. Petrusic - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):158-159.
  • Don't just sit there, optimise something.J. H. P. Paelinck - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):230-230.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What Ekman really said.Mats Olsson, Kathleen Harder & John C. Baird - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):157-158.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The place of psychophysics in the history of sensory science.David J. Murray - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):166-186.
  • The infinite regress of optimization.Philippe Mongin - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):229-230.
    A comment on Paul Schoemaker's target article in Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14 (1991), p. 205-215, "The Quest for Optimality: A Positive Heuristic of Science?" (https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00066140). This comment argues that the optimizing model of decision leads to an infinite regress, once internal costs of decision (i.e., information and computation costs) are duly taken into account.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mach's mechanics and absolute space and time.Sam Mitchell - 1993 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A 24 (4):565-583.
  • Two dynamic criteria for validating claims of optimality.Geoffrey F. Miller - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):228-229.
  • Complexity and optimality.Dauglas A. Miller & Steven W. Zucker - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):227-228.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Straining the word “optimal”.James E. Mazur - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):227-227.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Quantifying, valuing, choosing.Lawrence E. Marks - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):156-157.
  • Let's not promulgate either Fechner's erroneous algorithm or his unidimensional approach.R. Duncan Luce - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):155-156.
  • A parallel view of the history of psychophysics.Gregory R. Lockhead - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):154-155.
  • Fechner's theory of mental measurement.Stephen Link - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):153-154.
  • The example of psychology: Optimism, not optimality.Daniel S. Levine - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):225-226.
  • Derivation of Stevens's exponent from neurophysiological data.Artour N. Lebedev - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):152-153.
  • Why optimality is not worth arguing about.Stephen E. G. Lea - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):225-225.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Natural science, social science and optimality.Oleg Larichev - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):224-225.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The antecedents of signal detection theory.Donald Laming - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):151-152.
  • History of psychophysics: Some unanswered questions.Lester E. Krueger - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):149-150.
  • Types of optimality: Who is the steersman?Michael E. Hyland - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):223-224.
  • The chimera of psychological measurement.Gail A. Hornstein - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):148-149.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Optimality and constraint.David A. Helweg & Herbert L. Roitblat - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):222-223.
  • Fechner's impact for measurement theory.Michael Heidelberger - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):146-148.
  • Inner psychophysics, neurelectric function and perceptual theories.Stephen Handel - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):145-146.
  • Psychophysics, its history and ontology.Horst Gundlach - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):144-145.
  • The discovery of the psychophysical power law by Tobias Mayer in 1754 and the psychophysical hyperbolic law by Ewald Hering in 1874.Otto-Joachim Grüsser - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):142-144.
  • The head and tail of psychophysical algebra.Robert A. M. Gregson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):141-142.
  • From metaphysics to psychophysics and statistics.Gerd Gigerenzer - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):139-140.
  • Vaulting optimality.Peter Dayan & Jon Oberlander - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):221-222.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Organisms, scientists and optimality.Michael Davison - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):220-221.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Natural selection doesn't have goals, but it's the reason organisms do.Martin Daly - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):219-220.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation