Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. On one as an anaphor.Stephen Neale - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):353-354.
  • The child's trigger experience: Degree-0 learnability.David Lightfoot - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):321-334.
    According to a “selective” (as opposed to “instructive”) model of human language capacity, people come to know more than they experience. The discrepancy between experience and eventual capacity (the “poverty of the stimulus”) is bridged by genetically provided information. Hence any hypothesis about the linguistic genotype (or “Universal Grammar,” UG) has consequences for what experience is needed and what form people's mature capacities (or “grammars”) will take. This BBS target article discusses the “trigger experience,” that is, the experience that actually (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   93 citations  
  • Why degree-0?Wendy Wilkins - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):362-363.
  • Linguistic variation and learnability.Edwin Williams - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):363-364.
  • Learning Phonology With Substantive Bias: An Experimental and Computational Study of Velar Palatalization.Colin Wilson - 2006 - Cognitive Science 30 (5):945-982.
    There is an active debate within the field of phonology concerning the cognitive status of substantive phonetic factors such as ease of articulation and perceptual distinctiveness. A new framework is proposed in which substance acts as a bias, or prior, on phonological learning. Two experiments tested this framework with a method in which participants are first provided highly impoverished evidence of a new phonological pattern, and then tested on how they extend this pattern to novel contexts and novel sounds. Participants (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  • Why degree-0?Thomas Wasow - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):361-362.
  • Observing obsolescence.Nigel Vincent - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):360-361.
  • What's a trigger?Edward P. Stabler - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):358-360.
  • Data on language input: Incomprehensible omission indeed!Catherine E. Snow & Michael Tomasello - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):357-358.
  • Quasiregularity and Its Discontents: The Legacy of the Past Tense Debate.Mark S. Seidenberg & David C. Plaut - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (6):1190-1228.
    Rumelhart and McClelland's chapter about learning the past tense created a degree of controversy extraordinary even in the adversarial culture of modern science. It also stimulated a vast amount of research that advanced the understanding of the past tense, inflectional morphology in English and other languages, the nature of linguistic representations, relations between language and other phenomena such as reading and object recognition, the properties of artificial neural networks, and other topics. We examine the impact of the Rumelhart and McClelland (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Language acquisition: Dubious assumptions and a specious explanatory principle.I. M. Schlesinger - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):355-356.
  • Language acquisition in the absence of explicit negative evidence: how important is starting small?Douglas L. T. Rohde & David C. Plaut - 1999 - Cognition 72 (1):67-109.
  • On the format for parameters.Luigi Rizzi - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):355-356.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The rules versus similarity distinction.Emmanuel M. Pothos - 2005 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (1):1-14.
    The distinction between rules and similarity is central to our understanding of much of cognitive psychology. Two aspects of existing research have motivated the present work. First, in different cognitive psychology areas we typically see different conceptions of rules and similarity; for example, rules in language appear to be of a different kind compared to rules in categorization. Second, rules processes are typically modeled as separate from similarity ones; for example, in a learning experiment, rules and similarity influences would be (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Two perspectives on learnability.William O'Grady - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):354-355.
  • Language Acquisition Meets Language Evolution.Nick Chater & Morten H. Christiansen - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (7):1131-1157.
    Recent research suggests that language evolution is a process of cultural change, in which linguistic structures are shaped through repeated cycles of learning and use by domain-general mechanisms. This paper draws out the implications of this viewpoint for understanding the problem of language acquisition, which is cast in a new, and much more tractable, form. In essence, the child faces a problem of induction, where the objective is to coordinate with others (C-induction), rather than to model the structure of the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • Learnability considerations and the nature of trigger experiences in language acquisition.James L. Morgan - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):352-353.
  • INFL', Spec, and other fabulous beasts.James D. McCawley - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):350-352.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Negative evidence in language acquisition.Gary F. Marcus - 1993 - Cognition 46 (1):53-85.
  • Language acquisition in the absence of explicit negative evidence: can simple recurrent networks obviate the need for domain-specific learning devices?Gary F. Marcus - 1999 - Cognition 73 (3):293-296.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The true nature of the linguistic trigger.Marjorie Perlman Lorch - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):350-350.
  • Matching parameters to simple triggers.David Lightfoot - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):364-375.
  • The nature of triggering data.Howard Lasnik - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):349-350.
  • Language learning and language change.Anthony Kroch - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):348-349.
  • Does Universal Grammar exist?Jan Koster - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):347-348.
  • A possible mathematical specification of “degree-0” or “degree-0 plus a little” learnability.Aravind K. Joshi - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):345-347.
  • The Logical Problem of Language Acquisition: A Probabilistic Perspective.Anne S. Hsu & Nick Chater - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (6):972-1016.
    Natural language is full of patterns that appear to fit with general linguistic rules but are ungrammatical. There has been much debate over how children acquire these “linguistic restrictions,” and whether innate language knowledge is needed. Recently, it has been shown that restrictions in language can be learned asymptotically via probabilistic inference using the minimum description length (MDL) principle. Here, we extend the MDL approach to give a simple and practical methodology for estimating how much linguistic data are required to (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • The probabilistic analysis of language acquisition: Theoretical, computational, and experimental analysis.Anne S. Hsu, Nick Chater & Paul M. B. Vitányi - 2011 - Cognition 120 (3):380-390.
    No categories
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Language Learning From Positive Evidence, Reconsidered: A Simplicity-Based Approach.Anne S. Hsu, Nick Chater & Paul Vitányi - 2013 - Topics in Cognitive Science 5 (1):35-55.
    Children learn their native language by exposure to their linguistic and communicative environment, but apparently without requiring that their mistakes be corrected. Such learning from “positive evidence” has been viewed as raising “logical” problems for language acquisition. In particular, without correction, how is the child to recover from conjecturing an over-general grammar, which will be consistent with any sentence that the child hears? There have been many proposals concerning how this “logical problem” can be dissolved. In this study, we review (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Anatomia da Linguagem: Podemos Compreender Jogos de Linguagem a Partir de Redes Corticais?Inês Hipólito - 2017 - Kairos 18 (1):84-109.
    There is today much interest in research of neuronal substrata in metaphor processing. It has been suggested that the right hemisphere yields a key role in the comprehension of figurative language and, particularly, in metaphors. Figurative language is included in pragmatics, a branch of linguistics that researches the use of language, in opposition to the study of the system of language. There lingers, though, an open debate in respect to the identification of the specific aspects concerning semantics, as opposed to (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Degree-0 explanation.Roy Harris - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):344-345.
  • Language acquisition: What triggers what?Hubert Haider - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):343-344.
  • The language learner: A trigger-happy kid?Yosef Grodzinsky - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):342-343.
  • Positive and negative evidence in language acquistion.Jane Grimshaw & Steven Pinker - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):341-342.
  • Infinitely nested Chinese “black boxes”: Linguists and the search for Universal (innate) Grammar.Allen D. Grimshaw - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):339-340.
  • Zero-stimulation for parameter setting.Robin Freidin & A. Carlos Quicoli - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):338-339.
  • Two types of thought: Evidence from aphasia.Jules Davidoff - 2005 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (1):20-21.
    Evidence from aphasia is considered that leads to a distinction between abstract and concrete thought processes and hence for a distinction between rules and similarity. It is argued that perceptual classification is inherently a rule-following procedure and these rules are unable to be followed when a patient has difficulty with name comprehension and retrieval.
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Causality and parameter setting.Robin Clark - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):337-338.
  • Parameter setting in “instantaneous” and real-time acquisition.Guglielmo Cinque - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):336-336.
  • Language as shaped by the brain.Morten H. Christiansen & Nick Chater - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (5):489-509.
    It is widely assumed that human learning and the structure of human languages are intimately related. This relationship is frequently suggested to derive from a language-specific biological endowment, which encodes universal, but communicatively arbitrary, principles of language structure (a Universal Grammar or UG). How might such a UG have evolved? We argue that UG could not have arisen either by biological adaptation or non-adaptationist genetic processes, resulting in a logical problem of language evolution. Specifically, as the processes of language change (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   145 citations  
  • Brains, genes, and language evolution: A new synthesis.Morten H. Christiansen & Nick Chater - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (5):537-558.
    Our target article argued that a genetically specified Universal Grammar (UG), capturing arbitrary properties of languages, is not tenable on evolutionary grounds, and that the close fit between language and language learners arises because language is shaped by the brain, rather than the reverse. Few commentaries defend a genetically specified UG. Some commentators argue that we underestimate the importance of processes of cultural transmission; some propose additional cognitive and brain mechanisms that may constrain language and perhaps differentiate humans from nonhuman (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • On triggers.Hugh W. Buckingham - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):335-336.
  • Some observations on degree of learnability.C. L. Baker - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):334-335.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Empirical assessment of stimulus poverty arguments.Geoffrey K. Pullum - 2002 - Linguistic Review.