Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Is recursion language-specific? Evidence of recursive mechanisms in the structure of intentional action.Giuseppe Vicari & Mauro Adenzato - 2014 - Consciousness and Cognition 26:169-188.
    In their 2002 seminal paper Hauser, Chomsky and Fitch hypothesize that recursion is the only human-specific and language-specific mechanism of the faculty of language. While debate focused primarily on the meaning of recursion in the hypothesis and on the human-specific and syntax-specific character of recursion, the present work focuses on the claim that recursion is language-specific. We argue that there are recursive structures in the domain of motor intentionality by way of extending John R. Searle’s analysis of intentional action. We (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Rethinking the ontogeny of mindreading.Maurizio Tirassa, Francesca M. Bosco & Livia Colle - 2006 - Consciousness and Cognition 15 (1):197-217.
    We propose a mentalistic and nativist view of human early mental and social life and of the ontogeny of mindreading. We define the mental state of sharedness as the primitive, one-sided capability to take one's own mental states as mutually known to an i nteractant. We argue that this capability is an innate feature of the human mind, which the child uses to make a subjective sense of the world and of her actions. We argue that the child takes all (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • 1996 European Summer Meeting of the Association for Symbolic Logic.G. Mints, M. Otero, S. Ronchi Della Rocca & K. Segerberg - 1997 - Bulletin of Symbolic Logic 3 (2):242-277.
  • 1996 European Summer Meeting of the Association for Symbolic Logic.Daniel Lascar - 1997 - Bulletin of Symbolic Logic 3 (2):242-277.
  • Getting to Best: Efficiency versus Optimality in Negotiation.Elaine B. Hyder, Michael J. Prietula & Laurie R. Weingart - 2000 - Cognitive Science 24 (2):169-204.
    Negotiation between two individuals is a common task that typically involves two goals: maximize individual outcomes and obtain an agreement. However, research on the simplest negotiation tasks demonstrates that although naive subjects can be induced to improve their performance, they are often no more likely to achieve fully optimal solutions. The present study tested the prediction that a decrease in a particular type of argumentative behavior, substantiation, would result in an increase in optimal agreements. As substantiation behaviors depend primarily on (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Theory of Mind, pragmatics and the brain.Ivan Enrici, Bruno G. Bara & Mauro Adenzato - 2019 - Pragmatics and Cognition 26 (1):5-38.
    Theory of Mind(ToM) is a neurocognitive system that allows the perceiver to attribute mental states, such as intentions, beliefs, or feelings, to others’ actions. The aim of the present work is to analyse the engagement of the ToM system in communication, in particular, in communicative intention processing. To this aim, we propose anIntention Processing Network(IPN) with its own principles and mechanisms, that is, a brain network differentially engaged according to the complex intertwining of the context, goal, and action involved. According (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Interpersonal responsibilities and communicative intentions.Antonella Carassa & Marco Colombetti - 2014 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 13 (1):145-159.
    When they interact in everyday situations, people constantly create new fragments of social reality: they do so when they make promises or agreements, but also when they submit requests or answer questions, when they greet each other or express gratitude. This type of social reality ‘in the small,’ that we call interpersonal reality, is deontic in nature as all other kinds of social reality; what makes it somewhat special is that its deontology applies to the very same persons who create (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Biological Agency: Its Subjective Foundations and a Large-Scale Taxonomy.Adelina Brizio & Maurizio Tirassa - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Why Pragmatics and Theory of Mind Do Not Overlap.Francesca M. Bosco, Maurizio Tirassa & Ilaria Gabbatore - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
  • The Ability of Patients With Schizophrenia to Comprehend and Produce Sincere, Deceitful, and Ironic Communicative Intentions: The Role of Theory of Mind and Executive Functions.Francesca M. Bosco, Laura Berardinelli & Alberto Parola - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
  • Communicative-Pragmatic Treatment in Schizophrenia: A Pilot Study.Francesca M. Bosco, Ilaria Gabbatore, Luigi Gastaldo & Katiuscia Sacco - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
  • The Development of Anthropomorphism in Interaction: Intersubjectivity, Imagination, and Theory of Mind.Gabriella Airenti - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:401658.
    Human beings frequently attribute anthropomorphic features, motivations and behaviors to animals, artifacts, and natural phenomena. Historically, many interpretations of this attitude have been provided within different disciplines. What most interpretations have in common is distinguishing children’s manifestations of this attitude, which are considered “natural”, from adults’ occurrences, which must be explained by resorting to particular circumstances. In this article, I argue that anthropomorphism is not grounded in specific belief systems but rather in interaction. In interaction, a nonhuman entity assumes a (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Sharedness and privateness in human early social life.Maurizio Tirassa, Francesca M. Bosco & Livia Colle - 2006 - Tirassa, Maurizio and Bosco, Francesca M. And Colle, Livia (2006) Sharedness and Privateness in Human Early Social Life. [Journal (Paginated)].
    This research is concerned with the innate predispositions underlying human intentional communication. Human communication is currently defined as a circular and overt attempt to modify a partner's mental states. This requires each party involved to posse ss the ability to represent and understand the other's mental states, a capability which is commonly referred to as mindreading, or theory of mind (ToM). The relevant experimental literature agrees that no such capability is to be found in the human speci es at least (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Communicative competence and the architecture of the mind/brain.Maurizio Tirassa - 1999 - Brain and Language 68:419-441.
    Cognitive pragmatics is concerned with the mental processes involved in intentional communication. I discuss a few issues that may help clarify the relationship between this area and the broader cognitive science and the contribution that they give, or might give, to each other. Rather than dwelling on the many technicalities of the various theories of communication that have been advanced, I focus on the different conceptions of the nature and the architecture of the mind/brain that underlie them. My aims are, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Mental states in communication.Maurizio Tirassa - unknown
    Abstract. This paper is concerned with the mental processes involved in intentional communication. I describe an agent's cognitive architecture as the set of cognitive dynamics (i.e., sequences of mental states with contents) she may entertain. I then describe intentional communication as one such specific dynamics, arguing against the prevailing view that communication consists in playing a role in a socially shared script. The cognitive capabilities needed for such dynamics are midreading (i.e., the ability to reason upon another individual's mental states), (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • On the nature and role of intersubjectivity in communication.Maurizio Tirassa & Francesca M. Bosco - 2008 - In [Book Chapter].
    We outline a theory of human agency and communication and discuss the role that the capability to share (that is, intersubjectivity) plays in it. All the notions discussed are cast in a mentalistic and radically constructivist framework. We also introduce and discuss the relevant literature.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Sharedness as an innate basis for communication in the infant.Francesca M. Bosco & Maurizio Tirassa - 1998 - In M. A. Gernsbacher & S. J. Derry (eds.), Proceedings of the 20th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. pp. 162-166.
    From a cognitive perspective, intentional communication may be viewed as an agent's activity overtly aimed at modifying a partner's mental states. According to standard Gricean definitions, this requires each party to be able to ascribe mental states to the other, i.e., to entertain a so-called theory of mind. According to the relevant experimental literature, however, such capability does not appear before the third or fourth birthday; it would follow that children under that age should not be viewed as communicating agents. (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • A mentalist framework for linguistic and extralinguistic communication.Bruno G. Bara & Maurizio Tirassa - 2010 - Linguistic and Philosophical Investigations 9:182-193.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Collective goals and communicative action.R. Toumela - 2002 - Journal of Philosophical Research 28:29-64.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark